• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Family material hardship and chinese adolescents' problem behaviors: a moderated mediation analysis.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Share "Family material hardship and chinese adolescents' problem behaviors: a moderated mediation analysis."

Copied!
22
0
0

Texto

(1)

Family Material Hardship and Chinese

Adolescents

Problem Behaviors: A

Moderated Mediation Analysis

Wenqiang Sun1, Dongping Li2, Wei Zhang1*, Zhenzhou Bao1, Yanhui Wang3

1School of Psychology & Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou, China,2School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China,3School of Educational Science, Jiaying University, Meizhou, China

*zhangwei@scnu.edu.cn

Abstract

In the current study, we examined a moderated mediation model using the risk and resil-ience framework. Specifically, the impact of family material hardship on adolescent problem behaviors was examined in a Chinese sample; we used the family stress model framework to investigate parental depression and negative parenting as potential mediators of the rela-tion between family material hardship and adolescents’problem behaviors. In addition, based on resilience theory, we investigated adolescents’resilience as a potential protective factor in the development of their internalizing and externalizing problems. Participants in-cluded 1,419 Chinese adolescents (mean age = 15.38 years,SD= 1.79) and their primary caregivers. After controlling for covariates (age, gender, location of family residence, and primary caregiver), we found that parental depression and negative parenting mediated the association between family material hardship and adolescents’problem behaviors. Further-more, the adolescent resilience moderated the relationship between negative parenting and internalizing problems in a protective-stabilizing pattern; in addition, a protective-reac-tive pattern also emerged when adolescent resilience was examined as a moderator of the relationship between negative parenting and externalizing problems. These findings con-tribute to a comprehensive understanding of the mechanisms of risk and resilience in youth development. Moreover, the findings have important implications for the prevention of ado-lescent problem behaviors.

Introduction

Problem behaviors among adolescents residing in impoverished conditions continue to be of concern to developmentalists and policy makers. There is a substantial amount of literature in-dicating that poverty and co-factors are risk factors for the development of internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescents [1–6]. However, not all adolescents living in poverty de-velop problem behaviors [7,8]. Therefore, it is important to understand how and when poverty and co-factors operate as in the risk and protective processes.

a11111

OPEN ACCESS

Citation:Sun W, Li D, Zhang W, Bao Z, Wang Y (2015) Family Material Hardship and Chinese Adolescents’Problem Behaviors: A Moderated Mediation Analysis. PLoS ONE 10(5): e0128024. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0128024

Academic Editor:Xinguang Chen, University of Florida, UNITED STATES

Received:December 29, 2014

Accepted:April 21, 2015

Published:May 26, 2015

Copyright:© 2015 Sun et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of theCreative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability Statement:All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding:The authors received no specific funding for this work.

(2)

The current study was conceptualized from the risk and resilience frameworks [9,10]; spe-cifically, we integrated the family stress model (FSM) [2,11,12] and resilience theory [13–16] to examine two research questions. First, based on the FSM, we examined proximal family risk factors (parental depression and negative parenting) as potential mediators of the relation be-tween distal family risk factors (material hardship) and adolescent problem behaviors (inter-nalizing and exter(inter-nalizing problems). Second, based on resilience theory, we examined adolescents’resilience as a moderator of the relation between parental depression and negative parenting and adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems. The results of this study provide a better understanding of the risk and protective factors that influence the adjustment of Chinese adolescents residing in impoverished conditions, thereby offering valuable informa-tion about effective preveninforma-tion and interveninforma-tion methods [7,9].

Material hardship and adolescent problem behaviors

The majority of studies examining the relation between poverty and child development define poverty in terms of income; studies examining the influence of other dimensions of poverty, such as material hardship, are lacking [6]. Material hardship is a consumption-based indicator of eco-nomic well-being; it is based on the magnitude of financial hardship that families face, and in-cludes indicators of the ability to pay monthly bills, buy food, and pay for shelter [17–19]. Empirical evidence has shown that the distributions of material hardship and income are not par-allel; indeed, they are only moderately correlated [19–21]. Moreover, research has shown that families living in“near poor”households (with income ranging from 100% to 200% of the poverty threshold) also experienced one or more forms of material hardship, including not having enough food because of the inability to pay bills; thus, hardship is not limited to those living below the poverty line [18,19,20,22]. Indeed, it is clear that measuring poverty via income has limitations. Therefore, a growing number of researchers have begun to use measures of material hardship to study the association between consumption patterns or basic standards of living and children’s de-velopmental outcomes [17,18,23,24,25,26]. Therefore, material hardship was used as an indica-tor of family economic constraint in the current study.

A standard measure of material hardship does not presently exist; however, many research-ers have emphasized that one should be measured via indices of food availability, housing secu-rity, and the availability of medical care and financial conditions [18,19,20,25]. Gershoff et al.’s method for assessing material hardship was followed in the present study; specifically, Gershoff and colleague measure material hardship via four domains: food insecurity, housing problems, financial trouble, and insufficient health care.

There are several empirical studies that indicate that material hardship has a negative im-pact on children’s problem behaviors. For example, these relations were examined in a nation-ally representative sample of children in the United States [18]; it was reported that material hardship was associated with lower levels of child social-emotional competence (including in-ternalizing and exin-ternalizing problems). Similarly, in a longitudinal study, Zilanawala et al. [26] found that children residing in households experiencing material hardship scored signifi-cantly higher on internalizing and externalizing problems. These findings suggest that material hardship is an important predictor of children’s problem behaviors. Nonetheless, little is known about the mediating mechanisms underlying the relationship between material hard-ship and adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems.

The mediating roles of parental depression and negative parenting

Recently, researchers have acknowledged that the link between economic hardship and chil-dren’s problem behaviors is likely mediated by several factors; specifically, proximal factors

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(3)

likely mediate the relationship between economic hardship and children’s problem behaviors [1,3,6,27]. Indeed, Conger and colleagues [2,11,12] developed and tested the FSM of eco-nomic hardship. This model stipulates that ecoeco-nomic hardship indirectly and adversely affects children’s developmental outcomes through its impact on parents’psychological functioning (e.g., depression and anxiety) and behaviors (e.g., irritable, punitive, or rejecting parenting).

There is empirical support for this model among diverse racial and ethnic samples [2,18,

24]. For instance, in a sample of African American families, Conger and colleagues [2] found that economic hardship was positively related to caregivers’emotional distress; this was related to disrupted parenting practices that, in turn, predicted higher externalizing symptoms in chil-dren. Similarly, Mistry and colleagues [24] found that family stress processes were important mediators of the relationship between economic hardship and child behavior problems in a low-income, ethnically diverse sample. Therefore, it is clear that the FSM has been supported empirically across multiple studies.

However, these studies have exclusively focused on American children, who comprise less than 5% of the world’s population [28]. Indeed, evidence demonstrated that the link between socioeconomic status and child well-being varies as a function of geography and culture [1]. Because China is quite different from the United States in terms of economic and social securi-ty, families’experiences and responses may differ. To date, very few studies have examined the impact of poverty or material hardship on parents’well-being and children’s development with a Chinese population. In fact, approximately 11.8% of Chinese people (more than 100 million people) are living under the poverty line of annual income of RMB2,300 (about $375.55) [29]. Due to the large population in China, the number of adolescents living in poverty or near pov-erty is troubling. Therefore, in this study, we investigated the association between material hardship and adolescents’problem behaviors in a Chinese sample. Based on the FSM and prior research, we expected that material hardship would indirectly impact adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems via parental depression and negative parenting.

The moderating role of adolescents

resilience

Despite exposure to multiple family risk factors, not all adolescents who live in impoverished settings develop problem behaviors; thus, it seems that certain individual and/or contextual factors may ameliorate the relationship between risk factors and adolescent problem behaviors [8,9,13,14,16]. Resilience is the dynamic process where an individual is able to adapt positive-ly despite experiencing significant adversity. Therefore, resilience reflects a process of positive adaptation in the presence of risk that may be the result of individual factors, environmental factors, or the interplay between the two [14,15]. A key aspect of resilience is the presence of both risk and protective factors that either contribute to positive outcomes or mitigate negative outcomes. Protective factors have been identified as assets that are reside within an individual (e.g., competence, coping skills, and affect regulation) or resources that are external to an indi-vidual (e.g., support from family members and others) [13,16].

(4)

depicts a pattern where the correlation between a risk factor and symptoms of maladjustment is also significant when the level of a protective factor is low; however, the strength of the corre-lation is attenuated when the protective factor is high [13,14]. In summary, resilience theory describes a conceptual model that explains how youth overcome adversity. Moreover, this the-ory can be used to enhance individuals’strengths and help them build the positive aspects of their lives [13]. Importantly, there is empirical studies support for the protective models in studies of adolescent problem behaviors [31,32]. For example, in a sample of urban, African-American youth, Li and colleagues [31] reported that youth confidence significantly interacted with poverty in a protective-stabilizing fashion in the prediction of both internalizing and ex-ternalizing symptoms; in addition, a protective-reactive pattern emerged when the interaction between chronic hassles and family support was examined as a predictor of youths’

externalizing symptoms.

While the protective models of resilience have been examined, there is limited research on the mechanisms that may ameliorate the relation between family poverty and children’s prob-lem behaviors from the perspective of risk and resilience [6,9,10,33]. In addition, most of the previous work in this area has examined the buffering effect of a single protective factor [31,

32]; however, researchers have paid increasingly more attention to the effects of cumulative protective factors given that they have more protective power than a single protective factor [34–36]. Therefore, in the present study, the cumulative protection of resilience on youth prob-lem behaviors was tested by simultaneously examining individual power (goal planning, affect control, and positive thinking) and supportive power (family support and help-seeking). In summary, the current study expanded the FSM by examining the buffering effects of adoles-cents’resilience on internalizing and externalizing problems.

The Present Study

In the current study, we merged two frameworks (FSM, resilience theory) and examined the mechanisms that underlie the relation between family material hardship and adolescents’ inter-nalizing and exterinter-nalizing problems. Specifically, it was our aim to build on the existing empiri-cal research by simultaneously examining the mediating and moderating effects of family material hardship, parental depression, negative parenting, and resilience on adolescents’ inter-nalizing problem behaviors. The conceptual model and the hypothesized paths are depicted in

Fig 1. Specifically, based on the FSM, we hypothesized (Hypothesis A) that material hardship would be indirectly related to internalizing and externalizing problems mainly through its in-fluence on parental depression and negative parenting. That is, when family material hardship was high, parents would be at an increased risk for depression; this increase would be related to more negative parenting behaviors that would lead to greater adolescent internalizing and ex-ternalizing problem behaviors. Moreover, the model including direct paths from material hard-ship to other variables would best fit the data.

We also examined hypotheses base on resiliency theory; specifically, we expected that resil-ience would buffer the effects of parental depression and negative parenting on internalizing and externalizing problems (Hypothesis B). These hypothesized relations represent a moderat-ed mmoderat-ediation model [37], and the model proposes that the relationship between parental risks and adolescent problem behaviors is contingent on levels of adolescent resilience.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

The Human Research Ethics Committee of South China Normal University approved the re-search presented in this paper. All participants gave written, informed consent; they were

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(5)

informed of their right to discontinue participation at any time. Informed written consent was obtained from all adolescents and their primary caregivers; these procedures are consistent with the institutional guidelines of South China Normal University.

Participants

Participants were recruited from six public middle schools (three junior high schools and three high schools) in northern and southern China. The consent rate was above 95% in the partici-pating classrooms for adolescents and parents. After invalid questionnaires were eliminated (less than 3%), the total 1,419 adolescents (51% males) and their primary caregivers (58.8% of respondents are fathers, 37.6% of respondents are mothers and 3.7% of respondents are other caregivers) were used for the analysis. Adolescents mean age was 15.38 years (SD= 1.79). The majority of fathers (63.2%) and mothers (71.5%) did not complete high school; 10.8% of the fa-thers and 14.9% of the mofa-thers reported that they did not have a full-time job during the past year. The education levels of the parents were similar to those of the local and national popula-tions reported in the 2010 Chinese census data [38]. In addition, 21.8%, 16.2% and 62.0% of the students came from urban, suburban, and rural areas, respectively.

The data are available fromS1 File.

Measures

Family material hardship. Primary caregivers reported their family material hardship (food insecurity, housing problems, financial troubles, and insufficient health care) over the past 12 months [18,19,39].

Food insecurity was measured with the 18-item Core Food Security Module [40,41]. Six of the questions were rated on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 =never trueto 2 =often true(e.g., “We worried whether our food would run out before we got money to buy more”). The items were then dichotomized (0 =never; 1 =sometimes or often true) for the data analysis; this is Fig 1. Conceptualized model with hypothesis paths.

(6)

consistent with prior studies [25,42]. In addition, six questions were ratedyes= 1 orno= 2 (e.g.,“Did you ever eat less than you felt you should because there wasn’t enough money for food?”); these items were recoded (0 =no, 1 =yes), so that higher scores reflected increased food insecurity. There are also three preliminary questions that asked respondents whether they had skipped meals or eaten less at meals (e.g.,“Did you or other adults in the household ever cut the size of your meals or skip meals because there wasn’t enough money for food?”); participants responded eitheryes= 1 orno= 0. If they answeredyesto any of these questions, they were asked to respond to a follow-up question that asked them to rate how often they had cut/skipped meals for financial reasons. The follow-up question was rated on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 =only 1 or 2 monthsto 2 =almost every month; the responses to these questions were also dichotomized (0 =only 1 or 2 months; 1 =some months but not every month or almost every month) for the data analysis. The responses were averaged, with higher scores represent-ing greater food insecurity. The Cronbach’sαfor the present sample was 0.87.

Primary caregivers also indicated whether the family had lived in crowded conditions dur-ing the past year (0 =no, 1 =yes); if primary caregivers answeredyes, they were asked to rate the frequency on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 =only 1 or 2 monthsto 2 =almost every

month. This question was dichotomized (0 =only 1 or 2 months; 1 =some months but not every

monthoralmost every month) during the data analysis. Primary caregivers were also asked if

they had any maintenance problems in their home during the past year (e.g.,“Problems with pests such as rats, mice, roaches, or other insects,”or“Broken window glass or windows that can’t shut.”); this item was rated 0 =noor 1 =yes. The responses were averaged across the three items, with higher scores representing greater housing problems.

In addition, primary caregivers indicated whether the family had serious financial problems, or if they were unable to pay their monthly bills in the past year (0 =no, 1 =yes); if they an-sweredyes, primary caregivers then rated the frequency of financial problems on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 =only 1 or 2 monthsto 2 =almost every month. Their responses were di-chotomized (0 =only 1 or 2 months; 1 =some months but not every month or almost every month) during the data analysis. The responses were averaged across the two items, with higher scores representing greater financial trouble.

Two items measured insufficient health were combined [19,23]; each item was rated as 0 = noor 1 =yes. One item asked,“Was there anyone in your household who needed to see a doc-tor or go to the hospital but couldn’t because of the cost?”The other item asked,“Was there anyone in your household who needed to see a dentist but couldn’t because of the cost.”The re-sponses were averaged across the two items, with higher scores representing greater insufficient health care.

The mean scores of food insecurity, housing problems, financial trouble and insufficient health care were used as the four manifest indicators of the material hardship latent variable. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the four-factor model demonstrated a good fit to the data:χ2(269,N= 1,419) = 1508.830, CFI = .978, TLI = .975, RMSEA = .057, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.054, .060]. The Cronbach’sαfor material hardship was .89 in this sample.

Primary caregivers’depression. Primary caregivers reported their feelings of depression using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [43]. The CES-D Scale is a 20-item self-report measure designed to measure symptoms of depression within the last week; the measure was designed for use in non-clinical, adult samples. Respondents provided ordinal responses ranging from 0 (never or barely) to 3 (most or all of the time). The measure yields four factors: depressed affect (“felt sad”or“crying spells”), positive affect (“felt happy”or “hopeful about future”), somatic and retarded activity (“appetite poor”or“restless sleep”) and interpersonal problems (“people dislike me”or“people were unfriendly”). The items from the positive affect factor were reverse scored. Adequate test-retest reliability and construct validity

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(7)

have been reported for this measure [43,44]. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the four-factor model demonstrated a good fit to the data:χ2(162,N= 1,419) = 623.175, CFI = .937, TLI = .926, SRMR = .034, RMSEA = .045, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.041, .049]. The mean score of each factor was calculated and served as four manifest indicators of the pa-rental depression latent variable. The Cronbach’sαfor depression was .85 in the present study.

Negative parenting behaviors of primary caregivers. Primary caregivers completed the 12-item authoritarian parenting scale from the Parenting Style and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ) [45]. The twelve items reflect physical coercion (e.g.,“I slap my child when the child misbehaves”), verbal hostility (e.g.,“I explode in anger toward my child”) and non-reasoning/ punitive behavior (e.g.“I use threats as a punishment with little or no justification”). All items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 =neverto 5 =always. Adequate test-retest reliabil-ity and construct validreliabil-ity have been reported with this measure; it has been widely used in Chi-nese samples [45–47]. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the three-factor model adequately fit the data:χ2(51,N= 1,419) = 262.571, CFI = .952, TLI = .938, SRMR = .035, RMSEA = .054, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.048, .061]. The mean score of each factor was calculated and served as three manifest indicators of the negative parenting latent variable. The Cronbach’sαof negative parenting was .84 in the present study.

Resilience. Adolescent resilience was measured with the 27-item Resilience Scale for Chi-nese Adolescents [48]. This questionnaire assesses five aspects of resilience: (1) goal planning (e.g.,“I have a definite goal in my life”); (2) affect control (e.g.“Failure always makes me dis-couraged”); (3) positive thinking (e.g.,“Adversity is helpful for growth”); (4) family support (e.g.,“Parents always like to interfere with my ideas”); and (5) help-seeking (e.g.“When I’m in a difficult situation, I can’t find some people to rely on”). In addition, goal planning, affect con-trol and positive thinking reflect the higher-order factor of individual power; help-seeking and family support reflect the higher-order factor of supportive power. Participants rated each statement on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 =completely disagreeto 5 =completely agree. Ade-quate test-retest reliability and construct validity have been reported for this measure [48,49]. Higher-order confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the model adequately fit to the data: χ2(311,N= 1,419) = 1304.850, CFI = .898, TLI = .885, SRMR = .052, RMSEA = .047, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.045, .050]. The mean score of each higher-order factor was calcu-lated; these two factors served as two manifest indicators of the adolescent resilience latent vari-able. The Cronbach’sαfor resilience was .86 in the present sample.

Internalizing problems. Adolescent internalizing problems were measured with the 32-item internalizing problems scales of the Youth Self-Report (YSR) [50,51]. Adolescents re-ported their internalizing problems over the past 6 months on a 3-point Likert scale (0 =not

true, 1 =somewhat or sometimes true, and 2 =very true or often true). The YSR internalizing

scale comprises three subscales: withdrawal (e.g.,“underactive”or“secretive”), somatic com-plaints (e.g.,“feels dizzy”or“aches, pains”), and anxiety/depression (e.g.,“nervous, tensed”or “feels worthless”). Adequate test-retest reliability and construct validity have been reported for this scale [51,52]. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the three-factor model demon-strated good fit to the data:χ2(244,N= 1,419) = 988.867, CFI = .930, TLI = .921, SRMR = .039, RMSEA = .046, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.043, .049]. The mean score of each factor was calculated and served as three manifest indicators of the internalizing problems latent vari-able. The Cronbach’sαfor internalizing problems was .91 in the present study.

Externalizing problems. Adolescent externalizing problems were measured with the 28-item externalizing problems scales of the YSR [50,51]. Adolescents rated their externalizing problems over the past 6 months on a 3-point Likert scale (0 =not true, 1 =somewhat or

some-times true, and 2 =very true or often true). The YSR externalizing scale comprises two

(8)

aggressive behaviors (e.g.,“destroys own things”or“attacks people”). Adequate test-retest reli-ability and construct validity have been reported [51,52]. Confirmatory factor analysis indicat-ed that the two-factor model adequately fit the data:χ2(254,N= 1,419) = 1129.038, CFI = .887, TLI = .867, SRMR = .049, RMSEA = .049, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.046, .052]. The averaged score of each factor was calculated and served as the two manifest indicators of the ex-ternalizing problems latent variable. The Cronbach’sαfor externalizing problems was .87 in the present sample.

Covariates. Given that prior research has indicated that internalizing and externalizing problems varied by adolescent gender and age [53,54], these variables were included as covari-ates in the model. In addition, participant residence (urban, suburban and rural) and primary caregiver (father, mother or others) were also included as covariates; these covariates were dummy coded into two variables with city and father as the reference categories, respectively.

Procedure

Adolescents provided their primary caregivers with an explanatory statement and consent form. Parents who provided consent for their child to participate were required to return a signed consent form. Adolescents were also asked to complete a consent form according to the requirement of the ethics committee. The student-reported questionnaires were administered in classrooms by trained graduate students. The survey administrators explained the require-ments and the confidentiality procedures to all participants; the survey administrators also monitored survey completion. Students were given approximately 20 minutes to complete the questionnaires during class time; parent-report questionnaires were completed at home and were returned to the school in the following morning.

Analysis plan

There were small, non-significant intraclass correlations (ICC) among school levels for the in-ternalizing problems (ICC = 0.082, WaldZ= 1.504,p= 0.133) and externalizing problems (ICC = 0.047, WaldZ= 1.446,p= 0.148). Therefore, the analyses were performed at an individ-ual level [55].

The data were analyzed in the following steps. First, the expectation-maximization (EM) al-gorithm [56] in SPSS 20 was used to impute missing data; this procedure was appropriate given that the rate of missing scale items was less than 3.7% (the overall average was 0.39%). Second, structural equation modeling (SEM) was then performed in two steps using Mplus 7 [57]. Specifically, confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to evaluate the measurement model; then, SEM modeling was performed to test the mediating effects of parental risks (pa-rental depression and negative parenting) and moderating effects of adolescent resilience with the maximum likelihood estimation. Since some of the latent variable indicators were not nor-mally distributed (e.g., delinquency, interpersonal problems), bootstrap analysis was used to test the significance of the indirect effects. This calculation was repeated with 5,000 samples to yield a parameter estimate of both the total and specific indirect effects. If the 95% bias-corrected confidence interval for the parameter estimate was not contain zero, then the indirect effect was statistically significant, indicating a mediating effect [58]. In addition, the latent moderated structural equation (LMS) method was used to test the latent variable interaction [59]. In this procedure, a significant nonzero product term indicates the presence of an interac-tion. Simulation studies have demonstrated that the standard error estimates of LMS remain relatively unbiased even when some variables are non-normal [60].

Model fit was assessed using multiple fit indices, including the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(9)

standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). The model fit is considered adequate when CFI and TLI values are greater than .90, and the RMSEA and SRMR values are less than .08 [61,62].

However, when specifying a latent interaction term in a model, these model fit indices can-not be estimated because of a lack of a comparative model [63]. We followed the procedure de-scribed by Perren et al. [64]. Specifically, two models were tested. The first model was tested without the latent variable interaction (restricted model); the second model with the latent var-iable interaction (full model) was then tested. The nested model Likelihood Ratio Test [LR = -2 × (LogLRestricted- LogLFull)] was used to compare the difference between the two models. If the

first model had adequate overall model fit and the LR test found that adding latent variable in-teraction significantly improved model fit, then we could conclude that the second model had an adequate model fit. In addition, the metrics of latent variables were set by fixing their vari-ances at one, and the mean of each of the latent variables was fixed at zero by LMS [65] and LMS only offered an unstandardized solution.

In the mediating and moderating analysis, all of the latent variable indicators were centered to minimize multi-collinearity. Adolescent age, gender, primary caregiver, and family location were also included as covariates in the SEM analyses.

Results

Descriptive and preliminary analyses

The means, standard deviations, and correlations of all variables are displayed inTable 1. Gen-erally, indicators were related to one another within and across constructs; the correlations were in the expected directions. For example, all of the indicators of material hardship were sig-nificantly and positively correlated with the indicators of parental depression and negative par-enting. Similarly, the indicators of material hardship, parental depression, and negative parenting were significantly correlated with at least one of the indicators of adolescents’ inter-nalizing and exterinter-nalizing problems. The indicators of resilience were negatively associated with indicators of parental depression, negative parenting, and adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems.

In addition, the prevalence of internalizing and externalizing problems according to the clinically meaningful threshold (higher than 2 SD) [66] was 3.7% and 4.2%, respectively. In the present sample, 78.6% family experienced one or more of the indicators of material hardship.

Measurement model test

(10)

Table 1. The Correlations Between the Study Variables.

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24

1. Age —

2. Gender -.01 —

3. Mother -.07 -.12 —

4. Other .07 .01 -.15 —

5. Suburban .11 -.00 -.02 .01 —

6. Rural .04 .03 -.09 .01 -.56 —

7. Food Insecurity .14 -.05 -.04 .02 -.08 .22 —

8. Housing Problems .11 -.05 -.02 .06 -.05 .18 .49 —

9. Financial Trouble .12 -.06 -.06 -.00 -.04 .20 .51 .45 —

10. Insufficient Health Care .11 -.04 .01 .03 -.03 .07 .44 .37 .39 —

11. Depressed Affect .19 -.06 .07 .04 -.01 .08 .41 .31 .35 .35 —

12. Positive Affect .04 -.06 .00 -.01 .04 .01 .19 .11 .11 .16 .29 —

13. Somatic and Retarded Activity .14 -.01 .05 .03 -.02 .08 .41 .31 .35 .35 .74 .22 —

14. Interpersonal Problems .09 .01 -.01 .06 -.01 .04 .23 .20 .18 .16 .53 .25 .46 —

15. Physical Coercion .00 .08 .03 .04 .00 -.04 .18 .18 .15 .16 .30 .16 .29 .21 —

16. Verbal Hostility .03 .02 .06 -.00 .04 -.10 .11 .12 .12 .14 .26 .12 .28 .16 .58 —

17. Non-reasoning/Punitive .06 .08 .00 .05 .04 -.05 .17 .13 .11 .13 .32 .18 .33 .25 .55 .54 —

18. Individual Power -.13 .06 -.02 .02 -.04 .07 -.11 -.12 -.06 -.16 -.20 -.21 -.17 -.11 -.12 -.14 -.14 —

19. Supportive Power -.09 -.11 -.00 -.00 -.04 .07 -.11 -.10 -.05 -.11 -.16 -.17 -.19 -.12 -.23 -.25 -.27 .50 —

20. Withdrawal .29 -.05 .03 .03 .02 .03 .16 .13 .12 .18 .28 .11 .26 .17 .14 .19 .18 -.37 -.40 —

21. Somatic Complaints .13 -.12 .07 -.02 .02 -.00 .16 .14 .12 .22 .26 .15 .26 .15 .18 .16 .21 -.30 -.26 .50 —

22. Anxiety/Depression .18 -.09 .02 .03 .02 -.02 .15 .09 .11 .18 .29 .14 .27 .19 .18 .20 .24 -.46 -.42 .71 .58 —

23. Delinquency .13 .12 .02 .02 .03 -.07 .15 .06 .03 .12 .24 .14 .21 .22 .22 .21 .28 -.29 -.29 .38 .35 .44 —

24. Aggression .17 .00 .02 .03 .03 -.06 .15 .10 .10 .18 .26 .14 .24 .20 .22 .24 .28 -.39 -.32 .52 .47 .66 .62 —

M 15.38 .49 .38 .04 .16 .62 .15 .24 .22 .23 .35 1.04 .52 .20 1.70 2.29 1.64 3.56 3.53 .58 .42 .52 .15 .44

SD 1.79 .50 .48 .19 .37 .49 .19 .29 .34 .35 .44 .81 .46 .44 .64 .77 .65 .58 .70 .36 .34 .37 .19 .27

Note. N = 1,419. Gender was dummy coded: 0 = female and 1 = male. Primary caregiver was dummy coded into mother (= 1) and other (= 1), with father (= 0) as the reference category. Family location was dummy coded into suburban (= 1) and rural (= 1), with urban (= 0) as the reference category. Correlations were significant at the p<.05; the italicized values indicate that the correlations were no-significant at (p>.05).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0128024.t001

Family

Material

Hardsh

ip

and

Problem

Behavio

rs

PLOS

ONE

|DOI:10.137

1/journal.p

one.0128024

May

26,

2015

10

(11)

Testing the mediating effects of parental depression and negative

parenting

The 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals were used to test the significance of the direct, indirect, and total effects in the mediation model [33,58,67] since some of the latent variable indicators were not normally distributed (e.g., delinquency, interpersonal problems). Table 2. The Measurement Model: Latent Variable Factor Loadings.

Variables Standardized loading coefficients

Material Hardship

Food insecurity .80***

Housing problems .62***

Financial trouble .72***

Insufficient health care .57***

Parental Depression

Depressed affect .89***

Positive affect .32***

Somatic and retarded activity .83***

Interpersonal problems .58***

Negative Parenting

Physical coercion .76***

Verbal hostility .74***

Non-reasoning/Punitive .74***

Resilience

Individual power .72***

Supportive power .72***

Internalizing Problems

Withdrawal .78***

Somatic complaints .64***

Anxiety/Depression .92***

Externalizing Problems

Delinquency .67***

Aggression .93***

Note. ***p<.001.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0128024.t002

Table 3. Correlation Matrix for the Latent Variables.

Latent Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Material Hardship

2. Parental Depression .58*** —

3. Negative Parenting .27*** .46*** —

4. Resilience -.19*** -.29*** -.35*** —

5. Internalizing Problems .22*** .38*** .30*** -.67*** —

6. Externalizing Problems .20*** .33*** .37*** -.54*** .76*** —

Note. ***p<.001.

(12)

In step 1, the baseline model (covariates and material hardship) was used to examine the direct effect of material hardship on adolescent’s problem behaviors. The model fit the data well: χ2(66,N= 1,419) = 366.521, CFI = .940, TLI = .919, SRMR = .049, RMSEA = .057, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.051, .062]; this indicated that material hardship significantly and positively predicted adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems, with 9.6% and 8.5% of the variance explained for internalizing and externalizing problems, respectively. In step 2, negative parenting was added to the baseline model (the order in which negative parenting and parental depression were added to the baseline model did not influence the final model). This model also demonstrated a good fit to the data:χ2(108,N= 1,419) = 495.396, CFI = .941, TLI = .924, SRMR = .046, RMSEA = .050, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.046, .055], thereby in-dicating that negative parenting partially mediated the effects of material hardship on adoles-cents’internalizing and externalizing problems. The variance explained in the prediction of negative parenting, internalizing problems and externalizing problems was 7.4%, 16.1% and 17.2%, respectively. In step 3, parental depression was added to the model (saturated model) based on step 2. This model also fit the data well:χ2(178,N= 1,419) = 679.835, CFI = .944, TLI = .932, SRMR = .047, RMSEA = .045, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.041, .048]. The per-cent variance accounted for in the prediction of parental depression, negative parenting, inter-nalizing problems, and exterinter-nalizing problems was 34.1%, 21.1%, 19.2% and 18.6%,

respectively. However, path coefficients also indicated some non-significant links. Therefore, in order to develop the most parsimonious model, we eliminated the non-significant paths in the sequence of the saturated model and tested for differences in model fit. In the final model, we eliminated three paths: material hardship to internalizing problems, material hardship to externalizing problems and material hardship to negative parenting. The difference between the full model and final model was non-significant,Δχ2(3,N= 1,419) = 1.124,p= .771, suggest-ing that the full model offered no additional explanatory power over the more parsimonious one. The final model was found to fit the data well,χ2(181,N= 1,419) = 680.959, CFI = .944, TLI = .934, SRMR = .047, RMSEA = .044, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.041, .048]. The amount of variance accounted for in the prediction of parental depression, negative parenting, internalizing problems and externalizing problems was 34.2%, 21.2%, 19.2% and

18.6%, respectively.

As shown inFig 2, material hardship had a significant direct effect on parental depression (β= .59,p<.001), parental depression had a significant direct effect on negative parenting (β = .46,p<.001), and negative parenting had significant direct effects on internalizing problems (β= .19,p<.001) and externalizing problems (β= .27,p<.001). Parental depression had sig-nificant direct effects on internalizing problems (β= .26,p<.001) and externalizing problems (β= .19,p<0.01). In addition, the main effect of gender (β= -.22,p<.001) was significantly and negatively associated with internalizing problems, indicating that internalizing problems were more common among females than males. The main effects of age was also significantly associated with internalizing problems (β= .12,p<.001) and externalizing problems (β= .10, p<.001), indicating that older children experienced more problem behaviors.

Material hardship did not have a significant direct effect on internalizing problems, but there was a significant indirect effect (material hardship!parental depression! internaliz-ing problems,β= .15,p<.001; material hardship!parental depression!negative parenting !internalizing problems,β= .05,p<.001); and total effects (β= .20,p<.001). There was not a significant direct effect for the link between material hardship and externalizing problems; however, the indirect effect was significant (material hardship!parental depression! exter-nalizing problems,β= .11,p<.001; material hardship!parental depression!negative par-enting!externalizing problems,β= .07,p<.001); and total effects (β= .18,p<.001). These results partially supported our first hypothesis (Hypothesis A).

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(13)

Testing the moderating effect of adolescents

resilience

To test the hypothesized moderating effects of adolescents’resilience, two models were estimat-ed. In the first model, the main effects of resilience in the prediction of internalizing and exter-nalizing problems were added to the mediation model. This model had adequate model fit: χ2(221,N= 1,419) = 975.697, CFI = .925, TLI = .911, SRMR = .058, RMSEA = .049, a 90% RMSEA confidence interval [.046, .052]. The percent variance accounted for in the prediction of internalizing problems and externalizing problems improved to 46.7% and 32.3%, respec-tively. A second model was specified by adding two latent interaction effects (Parental Depression × Resilience, Negative Parenting × Resilience) to the first model. Likelihood ratio tests (the test statistic of the Loglikelihood is a distributed chi-square where the degrees of free-dom are equal to the difference in the number of model parameters) were used to compare whether inclusion of the interaction terms improved the model fit. The model with two latent interaction effects has a LogLFull= -9164.934, and the model without the interaction effect was

LogLRestricted= -9180.897. LR (df= 4) = 31.53,p<.001; the result suggest that including the

in-teraction effects improved the overall model fit. In addition, the latent inin-teraction effects of ma-terial hardship and resilience on internalizing and externalizing problems were not estimated simultaneously in the second model. This is because (a) the residual direct effects from material hardship to internalizing and externalizing problems were not significant in the mediating analysis, and (b) the model would become very complex and difficult to converge. Nonetheless, we conducted a supplementary analysis where only these interaction effects were included. The result indicated that adolescent resilience did not moderate the relationship between material hardship and internalizing problems (β= -0.06,p>.05) or the relationship between material hardship and externalizing problems (β= -0.11,p>.05).

As shown inFig 3, negative parenting and resilience exhibited significant negative interaction effect in the prediction of both internalizing problems (β= -.13,p<.01) and externalizing prob-lems (β= -.20,p<.01), thereby indicating that adolescents’resilience served as a buffer that miti-gates the adverse effects of negative parenting on problem behaviors. The latent interaction Fig 2. Standardized path estimates for parsimonious model with mediated effects.Age, gender, primary caregiver, and family location were included as covariates variable in this model, but is not shown in this figure. Curved arrows between internalizing and externalizing problems represent correlated errors.***p<.001.

(14)

explained an additional 1% and 3% of the variance in internalizing problems and externalizing problems, respectively. However, contrary to our expectation, the interaction between parental de-pression and resilience did not significantly predict internalizing problems (β= .01,p>.05) or ex-ternalizing problems (β= .01,p>.05). These results partially supported our second hypothesis (Hypothesis B). In addition, considering readers may be interested in the separate moderating ef-fects of the two high-order factors of the resilience, we also analyzed their efef-fects on problem be-haviors. The results were consistent with the moderating effects of total resilience.

For descriptive purposes, we used the procedures outlined by Muthén and Muthén (2012) in Mplus 7 to plot the predicted outcome variable by levels of the independent variable (range from -3SDto +3SD) at the high and low levels of the moderator (1SDabove the mean and 1 SDbelow the mean, respectively). Importantly, the latent variable indicators of internalizing problems and externalizing problems were centered in the moderation analysis; therefore, their values of them may be negative in Figs4and5. As shown inFig 4, for adolescents with low re-silience, higher negative parenting was significantly associated with higher internalizing prob-lems (simple slope = .24,p<.01). However, negative parenting was not significantly associated with internalizing problems for adolescents with high resilience (simple slope = -.01,p>.05). This result supported a protective-stabilizing pattern [13,14].Fig 5shows externalizing prob-lems as a function of negative parenting and adolescent resilience. The positive association be-tween negative parenting and externalizing problems was smaller for adolescents with high Fig 3. Unstandardized path estimates for structural model with latent interaction effects.Age, gender, primary caregiver, and family location were included as covariates in this model, but are not shown in this figure. Dashed lines represent nonsignificant latent interaction paths atp<.05. Curved arrows

among latent variables represent correlated errors.**p<.01,***p<.001. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0128024.g003

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(15)

resilience (simple slope = .10,p<.05) than for those with low resilience (simple slope = .37,p <.001). This finding supported a protective-reactive pattern [13,14].

Discussion

The mediating effects of parental depression and negative parenting

The mediation analyses conducted herein supported the work of Conger and colleagues’FSM [2,11,12] in a sample of Chinese adolescents and their parents. According to the FSM, eco-nomic strain is a grueling and demoralizing process that can lead to parents’depressed mood. In turn, this distress negatively affects parenting; furthermore, less nurturing and involved par-enting contributes to a host of psychological problems in children and adolescents [3,68,69]. In the present study, material hardship indirectly affected adolescents’internalizing and exter-nalizing problem behaviors through parental depression and negative parenting. That is, mate-rial hardship may have deteriorated parents’mental health by increasing their depressive symptoms; in turn, high levels of depressive symptoms lead to more negative parenting (e.g., verbal hostility, physical coercion, and non-reasoning). This finding supports previous research that indicates that material hardship negatively influences children’s adjustment through its adverse influence on parental mental health and parenting behaviors [18,24,70].

There were no direct associations between material hardship and adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors. This may have been due to measurement issues; specifi-cally, problem behaviors were self-reported by adolescents. Previous research has indicated that direct associations between economic disadvantage and children’s adjustment are com-monly found when primary caregivers, teachers, clinicians, and peers are the informants about Fig 4. Resilience as a moderator between negative parenting and internalizing problems that illustrates the protective-stabilizing pattern.

(16)

children’s mental health; however, when children report on their own mental health, indirect associations are often found, with paths between economic hardship and problem behaviors mediated through the actions of primary caregivers [68,71].

We also found that parental depression fully mediated the relation between material hard-ship and negative parenting. This result was consistent with some previous research [24,70]; however, other studies have not demonstrated the same pattern of results [17,18]. This incon-sistency in the literature may be due to the use of different measures in different studies. For ex-ample, Chien and Mistry’s study [70] included assessments of both positive parenting (e.g., parental warmth) and negative parenting (e.g., physical punishment). Although this requires further investigation, this finding highlights the critical role of parental depression in the rela-tion between negative parenting and material hardship.

In addition, parental depression affected adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems both directly and indirectly through negative parenting; this result was consistent with previous studies [72,73]. Furthermore, negative parenting was directly related to adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems [3,74]. Taken together, the findings of the current study replicated the main tenets of the FSM in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Furthermore, the results of the current study add to a growing body of literature examining the influence of proximal family fac-tors on the relation between material hardship and adolescents’problem behaviors.

The moderating effects of adolescent resilience

Previous studies on the FSM have focused on the mediating mechanisms that underlie the rela-tion between material hardship and child problem behaviors; however, little attenrela-tion has been paid to the heterogeneity of this relation [18,68]. In fact, many adolescents experiencing Fig 5. Resilience as a moderator between negative parenting and externalizing problems that illustrates the protective-reactive pattern.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0128024.g005

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(17)

material hardship did not have significant problem behaviors [7,8]. Therefore, it is likely that individual assets (e.g., goal planning, affect control, and positive thinking) and external re-sources (e.g., family support and help-seeking) may mitigate the deleterious effects of material hardship on youth problems [13]. The present study examined adolescents’resilience as a po-tential moderator of the relation between parental risk factors and adolescents’problem behav-iors. Our results suggest that adolescents’resilience is a vital protective factor in the

relationship between negative parenting and youth’s problems.

Consistent with previous research [31,32], the present study indicated that resilience mod-erated the relationship between negative parenting and adolescents’internalizing and external-izing problems. These findings indicated that individual development is complex and shaped by the interactions between the individual and environmental factors [75]. Specifically, a pro-tective-stabilizing pattern of resilience emerged when examining the relation between negative parenting and internalizing problems; this finding suggests that negative parenting was not sig-nificantly associated with internalizing problems for adolescents with high levels of resilience. This result was consistent with previous studies [31]. In addition, a protective-reactive pattern of resilience was found when examining the relation between negative parenting and external-izing problems; this suggests that the effect of negative parenting on externalexternal-izing problems re-mained significant (although weaker) for adolescents with high levels of resilience. This result was also consistent with previous studies [32]. Taken together, these different interaction pat-terns suggest that the adverse impact of negative parenting on externalizing problems is less likely to be buffered by adolescent resilience than internalizing problems. This finding provides a deeper understanding of the mechanisms that underlie adolescent problem behaviors; in ad-dition, this pattern of results also has important implications for the development of preven-tion and intervenpreven-tion programs for adolescents’internalizing and externalizing problems.

Contrary to our expectation, adolescent resilience did not moderate the detrimental effects of parental depression on internalizing or externalizing problems. This suggests that the associ-ations between parental depression and adolescent problem behaviors are more stable than the association between negative parenting and adolescent problem behaviors. This may be due to the fact that other factors influence the relation between parental depression and youth prob-lem behaviors. For example, we did not examine the potential influence of heritability on the relation between parental depression and problem behaviors [72,73]. Therefore, further re-search is needed to examine these relations.

Limitations and future directions

(18)

are more likely to use authoritarian parenting practices [72,76]; however, other parenting dimen-sions may also be relevant. Indeed, future studies should also include other parenting behaviors such as authoritative and permissive parenting. Fourth, the other parent who was not report on his/her depressive symptoms and negative parenting behaviors may also play an important role in the development of adolescents' problem behaviors; therefore, future study should examine simul-taneously the roles of father’s and mother’s mental health and parenting behaviors on adolescents' problem behaviors. Finally, the findings presented herein cannot be generalized to clinical popula-tions since the sample comprised a nonclinical population.

Practical implications

Despite these limitations, this study has important practical implications. First, based on the findings of the FSM, the reduction of material hardship will likely considerably reduce parental distress and youth problem behaviors [6]. Second, the mediating mechanisms of parental risk suggest that intervention strategies that focus on reducing parental depression and improving positive parenting behaviors are likely to be effective in reducing adolescent problem behaviors. Indeed, many interventions targeted at children in low-income families have been designed to implicitly or explicitly focus on processes that underlie the link between poverty and poor de-velopmental outcomes [77]. For example, Compas and colleagues implemented an interven-tion for parents with a history of major depressive disorder by increasing positive parenting practices; this intervention reduced children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms [78]. Third, the present study suggests that adolescents’resilience is a vital protective factor in miti-gating the development of internalizing and externalizing problems. Therefore, interventions may need to focus on developing assets and resources for adolescents exposed to material hard-ship. This will likely be as important as reducing risk factors.

In conclusion, the current study merged the FSM and resilience theory to address how and when family material hardship impacts Chinese adolescents’problem behaviors. The results validated the FSM in the context of Chinese culture. More importantly, the present study ex-tended the FSM by examining whether adolescents’resilience buffered the associations be-tween parental risks and youth problem behaviors. These findings suggest that adolescents’ resilience is a vital protective factor that mitigates youth problem behaviors in the contexts of material hardship. The moderated mediation model tested herein contributes to a more com-prehensive understanding of risk and resilience in youth development. Importantly, these find-ings have implications for the prevention of adolescent problem behaviors.

Supporting Information

S1 File. Data of the present study. (XLS)

Acknowledgments

We thank the schools, adolescents, and their primary caregivers for their support and generous contribution of time. We gratefully acknowledge Alison Pike and other two anonymous re-viewers who helped us improve the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: WS DL WZ. Performed the experiments: WS. Ana-lyzed the data: WS DL. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: WS DL. Wrote the paper: WS DL WZ ZB YW. Wrote the first draft of the paper: WS.

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(19)

References

1. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF (2002) Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol 53: 371–399. PMID:11752490

2. Conger RD, Wallace LE, Sun Y, Simons RL, McLoyd VC, Brody GH (2002) Economic pressure in Afri-can AmeriAfri-can families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Dev Psychol 38: 179–193. PMID:11881755

3. Conger RD, Donnellan MB (2007) An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu Rev Psychol 58: 175–199. PMID:16903807

4. Evans GW, Kim P (2013) Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Dev Per-spect 7: 43–48.

5. McLoyd VC (1998) Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am Psychol 53: 185–204. PMID:9491747

6. Yoshikawa H, Aber JL, Beardslee WR (2012) The effects of poverty on the mental, emotional, and be-havioral health of children and youth: Implications for prevention. Am Psychol 67: 272–284. doi:10. 1037/a0028015PMID:22583341

7. Compas BE, Reeslund KL (2009) Processes of risk and resilience during adolescence. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of adolescent psychology. Vol 1: Individual bases of adolscent devel-opment ( 3rd). New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. pp. 561–588.

8. Masten AS (2001) Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. Am Psychol 56: 227–238. PMID:11315249

9. Compas BE, Andreotti C (2013) Risk and resilience in child and adolescent psychopathology: Process-es of strProcess-ess, coping, and emotion regulation. In Beauchaine TP, Hinshaw SP, editors. Child and adolProcess-es- adoles-cent psychopathology. New York: Wiley. pp. 143–169.

10. Masten AS, Narayan AJ (2012) Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: Path-ways of risk and resilience. Annu Rev Psychol 63: 227–257. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100356PMID:21943168

11. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL, Whitbeck LB (1992). A family process model of economic hardship and adjustment of early adolescent boys. Child Dev 63: 526–541. PMID:1600820

12. Conger RD, Conger KJ, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL, Whitbeck LB (1993) Family economic stress and adjustment of early adolescent girls. Dev Psychol 29: 206–219.

13. Fergus S, Zimmerman MA (2005) Adolescent resilience: A framework for understanding healthy devel-opment in the face of risk. Annu Rev Public Health 26: 399–419. PMID:15760295

14. Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Dev 71: 543–562. PMID:10953923

15. Luthar SS (2006) Resilience in development: A synthesis of research across five decades. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation ( 2nd ed., Vol. 3). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. pp. 739–795.

16. Zimmerman MA, Stoddard SA, Eisman AB, Caldwell CH, Aiyer SM, Miller A (2013). Adolescent resil-ience: Promotive factors that inform prevention. Child Dev Perspect 7: 215–220.

17. Ashiabi GS, O'Neal KK (2007) Children's health status: Examining the associations among income poverty, material hardship, and parental factors. PLoS One 2: e940. PMID:17895981

18. Gershoff ET, Aber JL, Raver CC, Lennon MC (2007). Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models of income associations with parenting and child development. Child Dev 78: 70–95. PMID:17328694

19. Ouellette T, Buretein N, Long D, Beecroft E (2004). Measures of material hardship: Final report. Wash-ington, DC: Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, Office of the Secretary, US Department of Health and Human Services.

20. Boushey H, Brocht C, Gundersen B, Bernstein J (2001). Hardships in America: The real story of work-ing families. Washwork-ington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.

21. Mayer SE, Jencks C (1989). Poverty and the distribution of material hardship. J Hum Resour 24: 88–114.

22. Nelson G (2011). Measuring poverty: The official U.S. measure and material hardship. Poverty & Public Policy 3: article 5.

23. Beverly SG (2001). Material hardship in the United States: Evidence from the survey of income and pro-gram participation. Soc Work Res 25: 143–151.

(20)

25. Yoo JP, Slack KS, Holl JL (2009). Material hardship and the physical health of school-aged children in low-income households. Am J Public Health 99: 829–836. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2007.119776PMID: 18703452

26. Zilanawala A, Pilkauskas NV (2012). Material hardship and child socio-emotional behaviors: Differ-ences by types of hardship, timing, and duration. Child Youth Serv Rev 34: 814–825. PMID:22408284

27. Repetti R, Taylor SE, Seeman TE (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychol Bull 128: 330–366. PMID:11931522

28. Arnett JJ (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. Am Psychol 63: 602–614. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602PMID:18855491

29. UNDP in China. (2014). Eradicate extreme hunger and poverty where we are.http://www.cn.undp.org/ content/china/en/home/mdgoverview/overview/mdg1/.

30. Garmezy N, Masten AS, Tellegen A (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Dev 55: 97–111. PMID:6705637

31. Li ST, Nussbaum KM, Richards MH (2007). Risk and protective factors for urban African-American youth. Am J Community Psychol 39: 21–35. PMID:17380378

32. Gerard JM, Buehler C (2004). Cumulative environmental risk and youth maladjustment: The role of youth attributes. Child Dev 75: 1832–1849. PMID:15566383

33. Grant KE, Compas BE, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Gipson PY, Campbell AJ, et al. (2006). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Evidence of moderating and mediating effects. Clin Psy-chol Rev 26: 257–283. PMID:16364522

34. Ostaszewski K, Zimmerman MA (2006). The effects of cumulative risks and promotive factors on urban adolescent alcohol and other drug use: A longitudinal study of resiliency. Am J Community Psychol 38: 237–249. PMID:17004127

35. Schoon I. (2006). Risk and resilience: Adaptations in changing times. Cambridge, England: Cam-bridge University Press.

36. Sesma JA, Mannes M, Scales PC. (2013). Positive adaptation, resilience and the developmental as-sets framework. In: Goldstein S, Brooks RB, editors. Handbook of resilience in children ( 2nd). New York: Springer. pp. 427–442.

37. Edwards JR, Lambert LS (2007). Methods for integrating moderation and mediation: A general analyti-cal framework using moderated path analysis. Psychol Methods, 12, 1–22. PMID:17402809

38. Population Census Office under the State Council & Department of Population and Employment Statis-tics of National Bureau of StatisStatis-tics. (2012). Tabulation on the 2010 Population Census of the People’s Republic of China (Book I, Book II, Book III). Beijing: China Statistics Press. doi:10.3109/09286586. 2014.884603PMID:24568574

39. Heflin C, Sandberg J, Rafail P (2009). The structure of material hardship in US households: An exami-nation of the coherence behind common measures of well-being. Soc Probl 56: 746–764.

40. Bickel G, Nord M, Price C, Hamilton W, Cook J. (2000). Guide to measuring household food security, revised 2000. Alexandria, VA: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service.

41. Gundersen C, Kreider B (2009). Bounding the effects of food insecurity on children’s health outcomes. J Health Econ 28: 971–983. doi:10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.06.012PMID:19631399

42. Siefert K, Heflin CM, Corcoran ME, Williams DR (2001). Food insufficiency and the physical and mental health of low-income women. Women Health 32: 159–177. PMID:11459368

43. Radloff LS (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general popula-tion. Appl Psych Meas 1: 385–401.

44. Shafer AB (2006). Meta-analysis of the factor structures of four depression questionnaires: Beck, CES-D, Hamilton, and Zung. J Clin Psychol 62: 123–146. PMID:16287149

45. Robinson CC, Mandleco B, Olsen SF, Hart CH (2001). The parenting styles and dimensions question-naire (PSDQ). In: Perlmutter BF, Touliatos J, Holden GW, editors. Handbook of family measurement techniques: Vol. 3 Instruments & index. Thousand Oakes: Sage. pp. 319–321.

46. Zhou Q, Wang Y, Deng X, Eisenberg N, Wolchik SA, Tein JY (2008). Relations of parenting and tem-perament to Chinese children’s experience of negative life events, coping efficacy, and externalizing problems. Child Dev 79: 493–513. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01139.xPMID:18489409

47. Muhtadie L, Zhou Q, Eisenberg N, Wang Y (2013). Predicting internalizing problems in Chinese chil-dren: The unique and interactive effects of parenting and child temperament. Dev Psychopathol 25: 653–667. doi:10.1017/S0954579413000084PMID:23880383

48. Hu Y, Gan Y (2008). Development and psychometric validity of the Resilience Scale for Chinese Ado-lescents. Acta Psychologica Sinica 40: 902–912.

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

(21)

49. Wang X, Zhang D (2012). The change of junior middle school students’life satisfaction and the pro-spective effect of resilience: A two year of longitudinal study. Psychol Dev Education 28: 91–98.

50. Achenbach TM (1991b). Manual for the youth self-report and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Depratment of Psychiatry. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.10.011PMID:19171313

51. de Groot A, Koot HM, Verhulst FC. (1996). Cross-cultural generalizability of the Youth Self-Report and Teacher's Report Form cross-informant syndromes. J Abnorm Child Psych 24: 651–664. PMID: 8956089

52. Reitz E, Dekovic M, Meijer A M (2005). The structure and stability of externalizing and internalizing problem behavior during early adolescence. J Youth Adolescence 34: 577–588.

53. Grant KE, Compas BE, Stuhlmacher AF, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Halpert JA (2003). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychol Bull 129: 447–466. PMID:12784938

54. Lahey BB, Schwab-Stone M, Goodman SH, Waldman ID, Canino G, Rathouz PJ, et al. (2000). Age and gender differences in oppositional behavior and conduct problems: A cross-sectional household study of middle childhood and adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol 109: 488–503. PMID:11016118

55. Leech NL, Barett KC, Morgan GA (2011) IBM SPSS for intermediate statistics use and interpretation. New York: Routledge. pp. 223–256.

56. Schafer JL, Graham JW (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods 7: 147–177. PMID:12090408

57. Muthén LK, Muthén BO (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide ( 7th ed.). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. doi:10.1037/met0000028PMID:25822207

58. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing in-direct effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods 40: 879–891. PMID:18697684

59. Klein A, Moosbrugger H (2000). Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction effects with the LMS method. Psychometrika 65: 457–474.

60. Brandt H, Kelava A, Klein A (2014). A simulation study comparing recent approaches for the estimation of nonlinear effects in SEM under the condition of nonnormality. Struct Equ Modeling 21: 181–195.

61. Hu LT, Bentler PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling 6: 1–55.

62. Klein RB (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling ( 2rd ed.). New York: Guiford Press.

63. Klein A G, Muthén BO (2007). Quasi-maximum likelihood estimation of structural equation models with multiple interaction and quadratic effects. Multivar Behav Res 42: 647–673.

64. Perren S, Ettekal I, Ladd G (2013). The impact of peer victimization on later maladjustment: Mediating and moderating effects of hostile and self-blaming attributions. J Child Psychol Psyc 54: 46–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02618.xPMID:23057732

65. Kelava A, Werner CS, Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H, Zapf D, Ma Y, et al. (2011). Advanced nonlinear latent variable modeling: Distribution analytic LMS and QML estimators of interaction and quadratic effects. Struct Equ Modeling 18: 465–491.

66. Diler RS, Birmaher B, Axelson D, Goldstein B, Gill M, Strober M, et al. (2009). The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and the CBCL-bipolar phenotype are not useful in diagnosing pediatric bipolar disor-der. J Child Adol Psychopharm 19: 23–30.

67. Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological re-search: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51: 1173–1182. PMID:3806354

68. Conger RD, Ge X, Elder GH, Lorenz FO, Simons RL (1994). Economic stress, coercive family process, and developmental problems of adolescents. Child Dev 65: 541–561. PMID:8013239

69. Conger RD Conger KJ, Martin MJ (2010). Socioeconomic status, family processes, and individual de-velopment. J Marriage and Fam 72: 685–704. PMID:20676350

70. Chien NC, Mistry RS (2013). Geographic variations in cost of living: Associations with family and child well-being. Child Dev 84: 209–225. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01846.xPMID:22906161

71. McLoyd VC (2011). How money matters for children’s socioemotional adjustment: Family processes and parental investment. In: Carlo G, Crockett LJ., Carranza MA, editors. Health disparities in youth and families: Research and applications. New York: Springer. pp. 33–72.

(22)

73. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 14: 1–27. doi:10. 1007/s10567-010-0080-1PMID:21052833

74. Lim J, Wood BL, Miller BD, Simmens SJ (2011). Effects of paternal and maternal depressive symptoms on child internalizing symptoms and asthma disease activity: Mediation by interparental negativity and parenting. J Fam Psychol 25: 137–146. doi:10.1037/a0022452PMID:21355653

75. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris P (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In: Damon W, Lerner R, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Volume 1: Theoretical models of human development ( 6th). New York, NY: Wiley. pp. 793–828.

76. Evans GW (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. Am Psychol 59: 77–92. PMID:14992634

77. Munoz RF, Beardslee WR, Leykin Y (2012). Major depression can be prevented. Am Psychol 67: 285–295. doi:10.1037/a0027666PMID:22583342

78. Compas BE, Champion JE, Forehand R, Cole DA, Reeslund K, Fear J, et al. (2010). Coping and par-enting: Mediators of 12-month outcomes of a family group cognitive-behavioral preventive intervention with families of depressed parents. J Consult Clin Psych 78: 623–634. doi:10.1037/a0020459PMID: 20873898

Family Material Hardship and Problem Behaviors

Imagem

Fig 1. Conceptualized model with hypothesis paths.
Table 1. The Correlations Between the Study Variables. Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 1
Table 2. The Measurement Model: Latent Variable Factor Loadings.
Fig 2. Standardized path estimates for parsimonious model with mediated effects. Age, gender, primary caregiver, and family location were included as covariates variable in this model, but is not shown in this figure
+4

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Predictive multivariate model for the risk of malignancy in solid breast nodules based on the sonographic features, age and family history of a first- degree relative with

The adverse effect of negative comments about weight and shape from family and siblings on women at high risk for eating disorders. Prevention of eating disorders in at-risk

Similarmente acontece com o uso de etanol como álcool primário somente poderia falar de uma rota de transesterifação para produção de ésteres etílicos economicamente viáveis,

6. 9 &#34;Não acho certo classificar o infantil como tema romancístico porque cria confusões, uma vez que não são temas específicos de crianças, nem tratam deles nem são os

Based on analysis of the Family Health Program, which in 2011 started to be known as the Family Health Strategy, we identified considering percentage of health indicators

Although the literature has identified some risk factors for anxiety, depression and PTSD in critically ill patients and their family members, few studies have assessed the effect

In this article we concentrate on the analysis of the MADS-box multigene family of transcription factors that together with the APETALA2 (AP2) family are the key ele- ments of

It was shown that, whereas the risk factors for mortality during the first year of life were maternal age of 35 years or more and manual occupation of the family head, the main