• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”"

Copied!
8
0
0

Texto

(1)
(2)

3 2

Edição IPCA

ISBN: 978-989-97567-6-2

Design editorial · Cláudio Ferreira

COMMITTES

General chair

Paula Tavares

Organization chair

Pedro Mota Teixeira

Program chair

Alan Male

Organization

Paula Tavares, Pedro Mota Teixeira, Marta Madureira, Jorge Marques, Manuel Gaspar Albino, Cláudio Ferreira

Scientific Committee

Adrian Holme - Camberwell Coll. Arts (UK) Alan Male - Univ. College Falmouth (UK) Alan Young - Auckland University of Technology (NZ) Álvaro Barbosa - Univ. Católica (PT)

Ana Madeira Rodrigues - Univ. Lisboa (PT) André da Loba (PT)

António Costa Valente - Univ. Aveiro (PT) António Quadros Ferreira - Univ. Porto (PT) Carlos Nogueira - Univ. Nova Lisbon (PT) Chelo Matesanz - Univ. Vigo (ES) Debora Harty - Loughborough Univ. (UK) Eduardo Corte Real - IADE Lisbon (PT) Fernando Galrito - Inst. Polit. Leiria (PT) Francisco Laranjo - Univ. Porto (PT) Helena Barbosa - Univ. Aveiro (PT) James Walker - UCA (UK) João Manuel Ribeiro - IPCA (PT) Jorge Campos - Inst. Politécnico Porto (PT) Jorge Marques - IPCA (PT)

José Chavete Rodriguez - Univ. Vigo (ES) José Manuel Saraiva - ESAD (PT) José Antonio Castro - Univ. Vigo (ES) Juan Carlos Róman - Univ. Vigo (ES)

(3)

95

Definition of the main categories of

errors in “functional drawing”

Sahra Kunz.1

skunz@porto.ucp.pt

Abstract

Traditionally, teaching drawing has been mainly centered on an artistic (stylistic) approach. Drawing teachers base their work on methods similar to those used in the ine arts. Notions such as composition, style and expressiveness are primary in the trans-mission of knowledge to the class. Because of this approach, when learning drawing students sometimes ind themselves not quite grasping what they are being taught. This can be doubly true when students have not been trained in the techniques and materials of drawing, or if the main goal of drawing in their education is to provide a tool for other types of artistic creations.

I have conducted research within the classroom for the past years in the context of research for my doctoral thesis, collecting and organizing drawings. These studies took place in an environ-ment in which drawing is meant to serve as a functional tool or language for new media artists and technicians. Most students have little or no previous knowledge of drawing, and often experi-ence severe diiculties when confronted with even the simplest of tasks.

A questionnaire was used to provide information, such as cor-relations between age and drawing skills, and common mistakes in the representation of simple objects. This data lies at the basis of the attempt to catalogue and analyze main categories of errors in these students’ drawings; and to provide new educational tools for the drawing teacher.

1. CITAR – Centro de

Investigação em Ciên-cia e Tecnologia das Artes, Escola das Artes, Centro Regional do Porto da Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Rua Diogo Botelho, 1327, 4169-005 Porto, Portugal

Keywords

Drawing, Drawing errors,

inexperienced students,

functional drawing,

perspective

(4)

CONFIA . 2nd International Conference on Illustration & Animation Porto . Portugal . December 2013 . ISBN: 978-989-98241-6-4

Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”

Sahra Kunz 97

96

1 . Definition of drawing in this context

Recently, authors such as Betty Edwards [1] or John Willats [2] have advocated a new approach to teaching drawing. This approach relies mainly on the creation of a bridge between the evaluation of artistic abilities and of scientiic/objective knowl-edge. The notion of “artistic genius” is rejected in favor of a more encompassing perceptual and observational experience. An inter-crossing between the arts and the perceptual sciences encourages a student not only to blindly copy a motif, but also to understand the perceptual mechanisms that underlie his observation and representation of it.

In this context, one must proceed to deine drawing in a more speciic, and perhaps less “artistic” way. If one extends the deini-tion of a “good” drawing from that of a drawing with aesthetic qualities to a drawing with “functional” value, one can dissect the characteristics of this drawing in a much clearer way. For the pur-pose of this paper, drawing is characterized as a graphic language, whose message needs to be clearly transmitted from its creator to the person who interprets it. The advantage of drawing when compared to spoken or written languages is that the observer usually does not have to learn an alphabet or grammar, as his/her understanding of a drawing is immediate and usually unequivo-cal (if the drawing is executed correctly). This allows drawings to convey complex ideas and representations that become acces-sible to a wide audience, regardless of their linguistic or cultural background. The stricter deinition of this type of drawing should include the following parameters [3]:

Drawing seen not as an artistic activity, but as a means to convey, relect upon, or create the necessary foundation for other activities, be they of artistic nature or not;

Drawing that transcends personal artistic expression, executed in a way in which personal mannerisms and traits are largely absent (the opposite approach an artist would take);

Drawing as a tool used to represent a shape or scene in a cor-rect and rigorous way, keeping a strong similarity between the observed object or scene and its representation;

Drawing as a language, in which the amount of information present in a drawing should be adequate to the function it fulills; Drawing that is easily recognized and interpreted, meaning that

the mode of representation is clear utilizing the marks best suited in drawing;

Finally, and in order to simplify the scope of this analysis: Line drawing with no color, or shading.

These deinitions permit to narrow the study margin, and the identiication of some basic characteristics a drawing should pos-sess in order to be considered a functional tool. This becomes a quantitative analysis, rather than a qualitative one.

2 . Study group

The study that was conducted took place in an environment where most students have little or no previous knowledge of drawing, and in the beginning of their degree experience severe diiculties when confronted with even the simplest of tasks. They are irst year students of the undergraduate course in Sound and Image, at the Portuguese Catholic University. For them, the discipline of drawing is meant to serve as a provider of a functional language or tool, not for ine artists, but for new media artists and techni-cians. The Sound and Image course does not have the same entry requirements as a typical ine arts course, as its focus is divided between an artistic and technological approach. This implies that many students applying may never have drawn, or possess little knowledge of drawing techniques.

Altogether, 43 students participated in this study (of a larger group of 65), all of them enrolled in the irst year.

3 . Methodology

The methodology used to determine the main types of errors that both inexperienced and more experienced students made, took the form of a questionnaire [4], designed speciically to determine diverse aspects of background information and drawing skills. These errors were not yet organized into categories, but there were some suspicions of which they could be, that the question-naires were meant to clarify (as many errors conirmed through the use of the questionnaire had been observed in class over the years). This questionnaire had been used a similar format in previ-ous years (usually at the beginning of the irst year) to determine the general skill level of the students, and as such it was modiied several times over the years. For the purpose of this study, some

(5)

CONFIA . 2nd International Conference on Illustration & Animation Porto . Portugal . December 2013 . ISBN: 978-989-98241-6-4

Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”

Sahra Kunz 99

98

new questions and exercises were added, based on previous an-swers to older questionnaires.

The inal questionnaire consisted of two main parts, a written and a drawn one. In the written part, students answered several questions about their level of familiarity with drawing, their educational background, the frequency with which they draw, amongst others.

The drawn part consisted of three types of drawing exercises: from memory, in which students were asked to draw a seated per-son from a lateral, frontal and top view; copy from photographs, in which the students were asked to draw two slightly diferent views of a box and one view of a cylinder; and a copy drawing from a complex photograph of a human face, and of a cartoon character.

These questionnaires were handed out on the irst day of class of the irst year, before students had attended any drawing classes, and again on the last day of the second semester, at the end of the year, after they had attended drawing classes for two semesters. They were to be illed out in pencil, as the drawings were meant to be line drawings only, with as little shading as possible.

It was thought that these diferent types of questions covered the main problems that students seemed to exhibit during drawing classes, as they ranged from drawing from memory to obser-vational drawing, and drawing both simple and more complex objects.

4. Results

As a result of these questionnaires, (86 in total, 43 in each semes-ter), it was possible to collate a signiicant number of errors that

F.1 Overview of the

questionnaire handed out to the students

repeated themselves in each type of question. [5] The written part of the questionnaire allowed to ascertain that the study group was composed on average of slightly older students than usually apply to the irst year of university, that only a quarter of them came from an artistic study area in secondary school, that most (even those coming from artistic areas) of them didn’t draw outside of classes, and that most of them didn’t understand or apply the rules of perspective [6].

The irst part of the drawn part of the questionnaire asked

students to drawn a seated igure from memory from a lateral, front and top view, and in Fig. 2 one can observe a typical answer to this question. [7] It was found that in the lateral view, students mainly opted to represent the seated igure in a proile view, with little or no depth indications (sometimes one leg of the chair the igure was seated on was drawn as being slightly smaller than the one in the foreground). In the frontal view, many drawings showed a wholly frontal position, which led to increased diicul-ties in the representation of the legs, which should indicate some depth. In Fig. 2, the student opted for a less characteristic solution, twisting both the legs and the chair sideways, in order to solve the perspective problem. The top view also yielded interesting results, as comparatively to the drawing space used in the other views, the students mostly opted for a smaller, more distant looking draw-ing. This may refer to environmental stereotypes [8], in which a

F.2 A typical response

to one of the questions – student drawing from memory a seated per-son in a lateral, frontal and top view

F3. Example of student

drawing (copy from photograph) of two boxes and a cylinder

(6)

CONFIA . 2nd International Conference on Illustration & Animation Porto . Portugal . December 2013 . ISBN: 978-989-98241-6-4

Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”

Sahra Kunz 101

100

person observed from above usually is at a greater distance from its observer than a person observed from a side or frontal view.

The second part asked them to copy two boxes in slightly diferent perspectives from a photograph, and also a cylinder. [9] This question aimed to determine whether students were able to detect and represent subtle diferences in perspective. It was found that the irst drawing of the box was the one executed more correctly by most students. In the case of the second box, shown in a more unusual angle, a lot mot errors in proportion a perspective were detected. The drawings of the cylinder revealed that many students failed to understand its position, and in many cases drew inverted perspectives of it.

The inal part of the questionnaire consisted of a copy of a

human face from a photograph, and a copy of a cartoon drawing done only in black and white contour. [10] The drawings of the human face presented more technical challenges, as the copy was made from a heavily shaded photograph, and the face had many wrinkles. The example in Fig. 4 shows a typical example of the errors many students made, in which several elements of the face are left incomplete, as there is neither enough knowledge of hu-man anatomy to complete the parts that are in the shaded areas, nor the drawing skills to represent them. Also, a common drawing error, a discontinuous line is used throughout the drawing. [11] The drawing copying the cartoon had even more disastrous results [12], as the copy of a drawing presupposes even more knowledge of drawing techniques than a drawing made from life or from a photograph.

F.4 Examples of

stu-dent drawings (copy) of a human face and a cartoon character

5 . Types of errors

Based on the results obtained from the questionnaires it became clear that drawing errors stemmed from two larger categories of factors: Internal factors, which relate to the understanding and representation of the object, or from the knowledge of the principles and rules of drawing and from External factors, relating to lack of familiarity with drawing materials and the correct way to use them. [13] These factors also relate to what were termed Representational Skills and Drawing Skills. The irst relate to the internal thought processes that have to take place during drawing, such as the ability to compare the drawing to the drawn object, or the ability to observe an object with its formal characteristics and proportions in mind. The second relate to the manual skills needed to execute a drawing, both in handling drawing materials and the gestures needed to draw. [14] These observations led to the division of drawing errors into two sizable categories: Internal Representation Errors and Manual or Material Errors.

Internal Representation Errors stem from the incomprehen-sion of the observed object, of the viewpoint from which it is being observed, and of the perspective rules needed to draw it, and they can only be corrected through intensive drawing practice, and by learning and understanding the accepted rules of drawing, such as perspective.[15] Manual or Material Errors stem mainly from a lack of familiarity with drawing materials, and can occur in stu-dents with more experience if they are confronted with a drawing material they haven’t used before.[16] These errors are usually easier to correct than internal representation errors.

5.1 . Internal representation errors

Internal representation errors can be further subdivided into the following categories: Point of View or Rotation errors, Incompre-hension errors, Stereotypes and Relative Placement errors. Point of View or Rotation errors [17] can be characterized as errors in which one or more of the faces of an object are drawn with an excessive rotation towards the observer. This has the efect of making some of the objects features more visible than they actu-ally are from the observer’s viewpoint. This type of error is often more visible when the objects in the drawing are more geometric, but they can occur with any type of object. Also, this category of

(7)

CONFIA . 2nd International Conference on Illustration & Animation Porto . Portugal . December 2013 . ISBN: 978-989-98241-6-4

Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”

Sahra Kunz 103

102

error includes the errors identiied as Inverted Perspective errors and Limited Perspective. These include all situations in which an object is represented in a way in which its more distant elements appear to be larger that those closer to the observer.

In the questionnaire, the angles of the geometric objects to be copied from photographs were measured, and compared to those represented in the drawings of the students. In the rectangular objects, most rotation errors occurred in the top part of the object, with an accentuated tendency to show a larger visibility than was there. It was found that from the irst to the second questionnaire these errors tended to improve, as students attended drawing classes during a whole year. [18] Incomprehension errors [19] usually relate to more complex objects, such as the human face present in the questionnaire. In the case of these errors, a complex object is usually completed in an “imaginary” way, as those drawing it possess neither the drawing experience, nor suicient knowledge of the object in order to drawn it correctly.

Stereotypes [20] consist of modes of representation often ac-quired during childhood, which persist in adulthood if drawing is not a regularly practiced activity. They often manifest in drawings unconsciously, and are especially persistent when drawing famil-iar objects. Usually, when drawing stereotypes, most shapes are drawn as separate elements, rather than elements part of a whole

F.5 Example of a

student drawing presenting an Inverted Perspective error

F.6 Incomprehension

of the human face (student drawing)

F.7 Examples of

stereotypes present in the questionnaires (student drawings)

(for example, a face will consist of eyes, nose, mouth, eyebrows, etc. placed as individual objects rather than parts of the whole).

Relative Placement errors [21] happen when the transference of the observed object or scene to the sheet of paper causes prob-lems. They are especially visible when drawing a more complex scene, in which multiple objects have to be represented. Some-what similarly to Some-what happens with stereotypes, a complex scene is perceived as a group of distinct objects, rather than objects with concrete scale and placement relations between them.

5.2 Manual or Material errors

Manual or Material errors consist of Intermittent Line errors, Scale errors and Placement errors.

Intermittent Line errors [22] stem from a lack of knowledge of how the drawing implement (pen, pencil, brush etc.) is to be used in the context of drawing. These types of errors may have their origin in the diference between writing and drawing. Whereas in drawing it is beneicial to create a more continuous line, in writing the pen or pencil usually is used in a much diferent way, constant-ly lifting and pausing as each letter is created.

Scale errors [23] occur when, due to lack of experience, the limits of the sheet of paper are not understood or used correctly to represent a certain object or scene. The drawing is seen as being almost independent from the paper, and can be of inappropriate scale - too large or too small. This error is very well represented when larger objects are to be drawn, and inexperienced students struggle with scale issues.

Placement errors [24] are an extension of Scale errors, as

F.8 Drawings by the

same student in the irst and second ques-tionnaires – the use of Intermittent line is somewhat less obvious in the latter example

(8)

CONFIA . 2nd International Conference on Illustration & Animation Porto . Portugal . December 2013 . ISBN: 978-989-98241-6-4

Definition of the main categories of errors in “functional drawing”

Sahra Kunz 105

104

drawings will be placed at a random location on the sheet of paper. Even during copy works, these errors tend to appear, although they are graver in life drawing.

6 . Conclusion

Although in this study drawing is classiied as a tool, rather than an artistic form of expression, one believes that a more quantita-tive approach in the analysis of this discipline may beneit both those trying to teach and those trying to learn this vital instru-ment of creation. The knowledge and classiication of these errors can be an extremely valuable resource for the teacher in any classroom, as they allow for a targeted approach to the problems inexperienced students face when learning how to draw. It is believed that for a far too long time more technical questions in the learning of drawing have been ignored, in favor of an artistic and expressive approach. One can argue that both approaches are equally valid, and, if anything, should be used in a complementary way.

References

1. Edwards, B.: Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain. New York: Penguin Putnam Books (1999)

2. Willats, J.: Art and Representation – New Principles in the Analysis of Pictures. Princeton: Princeton University Press (1997) 3. Kunz, S.: Formas básicas de representación y ejercicios de aprendizaje en Dibujo - un estudio basado en la observación de alumnos adultos, UPV, Bilbao p. 35-36 (2011)

4. Ibid. p.84 5. Ibid. p.113 6. Ibid. p.133

F.9 Example of a

simultaneous Scale and Placement error in a drawing made by a student

7. Ibid. p.137

8. Willats, J.: Art and Representation - New Principles in the Analysis of Pictures. Princeton: Princeton University Press p. 175/p.294 (1997); Willats, J.: Making Sense of Children’s Draw-ings. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates p.90 (2005); Parini, P.: Operative criteria for stimulating imagination, fantasy and creativity. Speech, Monza p.7-8 (March 2001); Parini, P. Los Re-corridos de la Mirada, del Estereotipo a la Creatividad. Barcelona: Ediciones Paidos p.172 (2002); Marín, R.: Didáctica de la Edu-cación Artística. Madrid: Pearson EduEdu-cación p.283 (2003) 9. Kunz, S.: Formas básicas de representación y ejercicios de aprendizaje en Dibujo - un estudio basado en la observación de alumnos adultos, UPV, Bilbao p. 142 (2011)

10. Ibid. p.143 11. Ibid. p.144 12. Ibid. p. 146 13. Ibid. p.152 14. Ibid. p.253 15. Ibid. p.153 16. Ibid. p.170 17. Ibid. p.155 18. Ibid. p.230 19. Ibid. p.160 20. Ibid. p.162 21. Ibid. p.167 22. Ibid. p.170 23. Ibid. p.173 24. Ibid. p.174

Referências

Documentos relacionados

i) A condutividade da matriz vítrea diminui com o aumento do tempo de tratamento térmico (Fig.. 241 pequena quantidade de cristais existentes na amostra já provoca um efeito

This study aims to demonstrate that a modification of a chevron osteotomy, as popularized by Austin and Leventhen (1) , and largely employed for mild deformities, allows the

Abstract: As in ancient architecture of Greece and Rome there was an interconnection between picturesque and monumental forms of arts, in antique period in the architecture

The probability of attending school four our group of interest in this region increased by 6.5 percentage points after the expansion of the Bolsa Família program in 2007 and

didático e resolva as ​listas de exercícios (disponíveis no ​Classroom​) referentes às obras de Carlos Drummond de Andrade, João Guimarães Rosa, Machado de Assis,

Ousasse apontar algumas hipóteses para a solução desse problema público a partir do exposto dos autores usados como base para fundamentação teórica, da análise dos dados

This log must identify the roles of any sub-investigator and the person(s) who will be delegated other study- related tasks; such as CRF/EDC entry. Any changes to