• Nenhum resultado encontrado

The Impact of a Cooperative Method Embedded in a Writing Strategy Instructional Program on Student Engagement in School

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "The Impact of a Cooperative Method Embedded in a Writing Strategy Instructional Program on Student Engagement in School"

Copied!
9
0
0

Texto

(1)

w w w . e l s e v i e r . e s / p s i c o d

Original

The

Impact

of

a

Cooperative

Method

Embedded

in

a

Writing

Strategy

Instructional

Program

on

Student

Engagement

in

School

夽,夽夽

Maria

José

Prata

a,∗

,

Isabel

Festas

a

,

Albertina

L.

Oliveira

a

,

and

Feliciano

H.

Veiga

b

aFacultyofPsychologyandofSciencesofEducation,UniversityofCoimbra,RuadoColégioNovo,Apartado6153,3001-802Coimbra,Portugal bInstitutodeEducac¸ãodaUniversidadedeLisboa,AlamedadaUniversidade,1649-013Lisboa,Portugal

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory: Received29June2018 Accepted21November2018 Availableonline9January2019

Keywords:

Studentengagementinschool Cooperativemethods Writing

SRSD

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Cooperativemethodsareapowerfultoolforimprovingbothstudentengagementinschoolandwriting competence.Thisstudyexaminedtheeffectsofacooperativemethodembeddedinawritingstrategy instructionalprogramonstudentengagementinschool,namelyontheircognitive,affective,behavioral, andpersonalagencydimensions.UsingtheStudents’EngagementinSchool:Four-dimensionalScale (SES-4DS),213ninthgradestudents(from14to17yearsold)wereevaluatedbeforeandaftertheintervention. Twoconditionswerecreated:anexperimentalgroupwhereacooperativemethodassociatedwitha writ-ingstrategyinstructionalprogramwasimplementedandacontrolgroupwherestudentswereinstructed withawritingstrategyinstructionalprogram.Overasix-monthperiod,theexperimentalstudents par-ticipatedinacooperativeexperienceembeddedinawritingstrategyinstructionalprogram,whereasthe controlstudentswereinstructedwithwritingstrategymethod.Resultsindicatedthat,whencompared withthecontrol,theexperimentalstudentsincreasedtheirlevelsofbehavioralandaffective engage-mentbutnottheirlevelsofcognitiveengagement,personalagency,andtotalengagement.Implications oftheseresults,limitationsanddirectionsforfutureresearchareanalyzed,anddiscussed.

©2018PublishedbyElsevierEspa ˜na,S.L.U.onbehalfofUniversidaddePa´ısVasco.

El

impacto

de

un

método

cooperativo

integrado

en

un

programa

instruccional

de

estrategias

de

escritura

en

la

implicación

del

alumnado

en

la

escuela

Palabrasclave: Motivaciónescolar Métodoscooperativos Escritura ProgramaSRSD

r

e

s

u

m

e

n

Losmétodoscooperativossonunaherramientapoderosatantoparamejorarlamotivaciónescolardel alumnadocomosucompetenciadeescritura.Esteestudioexaminalosefectosdeunmétodocooperativo integradoenunprogramadeestrategiasdeense ˜nanzadelaescrituraenlamotivaciónacadémicadelos estudiantes(dimensionescognitiva,afectiva,conductualeimplicaciónpersonal).Sehaaplicadolaescala MotivaciónAcadémicadelosEstudiantes(SES-4DS)a213alumnosdel9◦gradodeescolaridad(de14a

17a ˜nos),divididosendosgrupos(experimentalycontrol)quehansidoevaluadosantesydespuésdela intervención.Duranteunperíododeseismeseselgrupoexperimentalhasidosometidoaunprograma deestrategiasdeense ˜nanzadelaescritura,seguidodeunmétodocooperativo;elgrupodecontrolsólo fuesujetoalprogramadeestrategiasdeense ˜nanzadelaescritura.Losresultadosindicanqueelgrupo experimental,encomparaciónconelgrupodecontrol,haaumentadosignificativamentesumotivación escolarenlasdimensionescomportamentalyafectiva,peronoenlasdimensionescognitivaeimplicación personal,nienlamotivaciónglobal.Seanalizanysediscutenlasimplicacionesdeestosresultados,así comolaslimitacionesdelestudioylasrecomendacionesparainvestigacionesfuturas.

©2018PublicadoporElsevierEspa ˜na,S.L.U.ennombredeUniversidaddePa´ısVasco.

PIIoforiginalarticle:S1136-1034(18)30168-0.

夽 Pleasecitethisarticleas:PrataMJ,FestasI,Oliveira,AL,VeigaF.Elimpactodeunmétodocooperativointegradoenunprogramainstruccionaldeestrategiasdeescritura enlaimplicacióndelalumnadoenlaescuela.ReviPsicodidáct.2019;24:145–153.

夽夽 ThisstudywasmadepossiblebyagrantfromtheFundac¸ãoparaaCiênciaeTecnologia(ScienceandTechnologyFoundation),Lisboa,Portugal(SFRH/BD/84264/2012) andbyfundingforResearchandDevelopmentinEducationgrant(UID/CED/04107/2016)fromtheFundac¸ãoparaaCiênciaeTecnologia(ScienceandTechnologyFoundation), Lisboa,Portugal.

∗ Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddress:mariab.prata@gmail.com(M.J.Prata).

(2)

Introduction

Thewayinwhich studentsareinvolvedinschools –student engagementinschool –is acrucial factortotheiracademic and personalsuccessandisseenasameanstoaddressmanyofthe problems which students experience and which affect schools

(Appleton, Christenson,&Furlong,2008;Hagenauer,Hascher,&

Volet,2015).

Becauseacademiclearningrequireseffort,studentengagement isessentialinthatitleadstosustainedstudyandsupports activ-ities needed for students to improve theiracademic work and developproficiency. Increasedproficiencyin academictasks, in turn,leadstogreaterstudentengagementbecausecompetenceis necessarytokeepstudentsmotivatedtoengageinschoolactivities

(Irvin,Meltzer,&Dukes,2007).Severalstudieshaveshownthat

therelationshipbetweenengagementandacademiccompetenceis bidirectional(Miranda-Zapata,Lara,Navarro,Saracostti,&de-Toro,

2018; Wonglorsaichon, Wongwanich, & Wiratchai, 2014). This

meansthatthemorestudentsareengagedinschool,themorethey learn,andthemoreacademicallycompetentandsuccessfulthey arethemoreworthwhiletheyfeel,whichinturn,increasestheir engagement(Wonglorsaichonetal.,2014).Thus,ifengagement isanessentialconditiontoacademicsuccess,insimilarfashion, increasingstudents’proficiencyshoulddevelopengagement. Profi-ciencyinwritingisdeemedparticularlyimportantbecausewriting isacoreliteracyskill(Fidalgo,Harris,&Braaksma,2017;

Gutiérrez-Fresneda,2018).However,beinga difficulttask, students often

viewwritingasfrustratingandoverwhelming(Zumbrunn,Marrs,

&Mewborn,2016).Thisjustifiesthepursuitofoptimalmeansthat

wouldencouragegreaterengagementfromelementaryand mid-dleschoolstudentsinsuchanessentialacademicarea.Cooperative methodshavebeensuggested asanappropriate answertothis problem,asresearchhasshownthattheyrepresentapowerfultool notonlyforimprovingwritingcompetence(VanSteendam,2016) butalsoforincreasingengagement(Wentzel,2009)andacademic proficiency(Bommarito,2015).

Studentengagementinschool

Studentengagementinschoolisconcernedwiththeextentof studentsinvolvementinschool,andthereforewiththeir motiva-tiontolearn(Simon-Morton&Chen,2009);inaddition,itisrelated toschoolperformanceandtoanappropriatestudentsbehavior,and evenwithpositiveandnegativeteacheremotions(Hagenaueretal.,

2015;Wonglorsaichonetal.,2014).It isknownthatbehavioral

problemsandtheriskofschooldropoutareoftenassociatedwith lowlevelsofstudentengagement(Appletonetal.,2008;Fredricks,

Blumenfeld,&Paris,2004;Reeve&Tseng,2011).

Theliteraturepointstoengagementashavinga multidimen-sional nature, including four dynamically related dimensions: cognitive, affective, behavioral, and personal agency (Reeve &

Tseng,2011).Thecognitivedimensioncoversstudents’personal

investment(Ainley,1993),learningapproachesandself-regulatory strategies (Fredricks etal., 2004).In addition,it is operational-izedastheperceptions andthebeliefsabouttheself,aboutthe school,andaboutthecolleagues,anditincludesself-efficacy strate-gies,motivationandacademicaspirations (Jimerson,Campos,&

Greif,2003).Theaffectiveorpsychologicaldimension(Appleton

etal.,2008)referstothesenseofidentificationwithschool,to emotionsgeneratedbyschool,colleaguesandteachers,andtothe senseofbelongingtoschool(Johnson,Crosnoe,&Elder,2001).The behavioraldimensionisrelatedtoactionsandpracticesdirected towardschool,includingmanypositivebehaviors,suchasdoing homework,payingattentionduringlessons(Johnsonetal.,2001), participatinginacademictasksandobtaininggoodgrades(Jordan

&Nettles,2000), involvementin extra-curricularactivities, and

respectingschoolnorms(Fredricksetal.,2004;Gutiérrez,Tomás,

Romero,&Barrica,2017).Personalagencyisdefinedasstudents’

constructive contributionto the course of the instruction they receive(Reeve&Tseng,2011).

Studentengagementinschoolisinfluencedbypersonal vari-ables, such as self-efficacy, self-reliance, and self-concept, and contextual variables, such as family, peers, and school (Veiga, 2016).Furthermore,findingsfromtheUnitedStates,Portugalor Spainconsistentlyrevealedthatkeyschoolattributessuchas self-concept(Martínez,Cruise, García,& Murgui,2017), self-esteem

(Rodrigues, Veiga, Fuentes, & García, 2013), self-reliance,

psy-chologicaladjustment(Fuentes,García,Gracia,&Alarcón,2015), or bullying victimization (traditional bullying and

cyberbully-ing; Martínez,Murgui,Garcia, &Garcia, 2019)are related with

schoolcontextbutalsowithparents’andpeers’influence.Thereis evidencethatteachersupporthasaninfluenceonstudent engage-ment,academicsuccess, andsatisfaction withschool(Gutiérrez etal.,2017), andthatschoolengagementhasadirect effecton attendanceto classes and school performance (Miranda-Zapata

etal.,2018).

In this paper we are particularly interested in contextual variables,namelythoserelated toschool.Severalschool-related variables,suchasthesocial climate, and instructionalpractices have been shown to have an impact on student engagement

(Patrick,Ryan,&Kaplan,2007;Ryan&Patrick,2001).Thebeliefs

and behaviors of teachers, along with theirsupportof student autonomy help to create a learning climate favorable to the practiceof decision makingand self-regulating abilities,one in whichthestudentsengageinschoolactivities(Roeser,Eccles,&

Sameroff,2000;Ryan&Patrick,2001).Instructionalpracticesare

also strongly related to student engagement, in that sustained engagementdependsgreatlyontheinstructionneededtosupport studentsintheireffortstolearn.Instructionalpracticesthathave beenproventobepowerful,suchascoaching,scaffolding, mod-eling,andfeedbackconstituteprecioussupporttohelpstudents sustaintheirengagementintasks(Irvinetal.,2007).Cooperative methods,inparticular,areregardedasoneofthemostimportant facilitatorsofengagementinlearningactivities(cf.Wentzel,2009). Cooperativemethods

Cooperativelearningisbasedsignificantlyonsocial interdepen-dencetheory(Johnson&Johnson,1999;Johnson,Johnson,&Smith,

2007).JohnsonandJohnson(1999,2009,2016)defined

cooper-ativelearningashavingcertainessentialcharacteristics,suchas positive interdependence,individualaccountability, face-to-face promotiveinteraction,and appropriateuseofsocial skills. Posi-tiveinterdependenceoccurs whentheinterconnection between eachmemberresultsinthemutualaccomplishmentofacommon task.Accordingtopositiveinterdependence,individualstudents shouldunderstandthattheachievementofgroupgoalsisessential forattainingtheirownobjectives.Individualaccountabilityis con-cernedwiththeresponsibilitythateachstudentmustassumein ordertopursuethegroup’sgoals.Thelearningsuccessofa coop-erativegroupdependsoneachgroupmember’slearningsuccess, andnostudentisabletoperformhis/herpartifapeerfailsto ful-fillhis/her individualresponsibilityrelatedtothecommontask

(Donnell,Hmelo-Silver, &Erkens,2013).Face-to-facepromotive

interactionsignifiesthatindividualsassist eachotherefficiently andfacilitateeachother’seffortstoperformeachpartsothatthe commontaskcanbeaccomplished.Theappropriateuseofsocial skillsisanessentialconditionforworkingingroups.Since cooper-ativelearningrequiresgroupmemberstolearnboththeacademic subjectsandtheinterpersonalskillsensurestheeffective opera-tionofgroupsworkingcooperatively.Severalcooperativemethods have been developed, with the jigsaw being one of the most

(3)

popular(Aronson,Stephan,Sikes,Blaney,&Snapp,1978).Inthe jigsawmethod,teachersorganizethestudentsintosmallgroups ofthreeorfourmembers(thehomegroup),andassigneach mem-berapartofacommontask(A,B,C,D).Afterwards,studentswith thesamepartmeetintheexpertgroupstostudyit.Inthethird step,thestudentswhoareexpertsintheirrespectivepartreturnto theirhomegroupstoshareinformationandtoworktogetheronthe commontask.Ajigsawcycleendswiththeindividualachievement ofataskrelatedtotheinitialcommontask(Johnson&Johnson,

1999).

Ascooperativemethods concentrateonpositive interdepen-dence,individualaccountability,face-to-facepromotive interac-tion,andappropriateuseofsocialskills,theysustainthepositive learning climate.Research hasshown thatcooperative learning in structured groups decreases competitive verbal and non-verbalbehaviors(Sharan,1999),andincreasestolerancebetween students.Furthermore,it increasesassistanceand positive rela-tionshipsbetweenstudents(Gillies,2017),andprosocialbehaviors, namelyintheactivitiesofhelpandcooperation(Carrasco,Alarcón,

&Trianes,2018).Structuredcooperativelearningexperiences

pro-motes help giving behaviors to succeed when students feel a senseofgroupcohesion(Johnson&Johnson,2016;Slavin,2014), increasesteampotency,thatis,confidenceintheteam(Leóndel

Barcoetal.,2017),andleadstoagreateracceptanceofstudents

withspecialneeds(Gillies,2007;Vedder&Veendrick,2003).In addition,cooperativemethods,andparticularlythejigsaw, repre-sentapowerfultooltomaintainstudentengagementinlearning activities(cf.Buijs&Admiraal,2013),resultinginincreased aca-demicproficiency(cf.Irvinetal.,2007;Wentzel,2009).Several studieshavedemonstratedtheadvantagesofcooperativelearning structuresovercompetitiveandindividualisticonesonacademic performance(e.g.,Johnsonetal.,2007;Roseth,Johnson,&Johnson,

2008).

The intellectual benefits of cooperative learning serves the criticalfunctionofdemandingtheothersbeheard,butitraises alsosocialskills,andchildren’scuriosity,interest,andconfidence

(Kuhn,2015).Cooperativemethodsrequirethesynchronous

com-binationoftheintellectualeffortsofeachindividualineachofthe stepsthatmakeupthelearningtask(Todd&Dadlani,2014).Forall thesereasons,cooperativemethodsinfluencenotonlythe learn-ingclimatebutalsotheacademiccompetence,withbothofthem beingessentialtopromotestudentsengagement.

Writing

Writing literacy playsa crucial role in academicproficiency (e.g., Klein, Boscolo, Kirkpatrick, & Gelati, 2014).Writing is an important meantodemonstrate and toproduce knowledge,to gatherandrecallinformation,toimprovestudents’abilitytolearn, andtopromoteschoolsuccess(e.g.,Klein,Arcon,&Baker,2016). However,writingisacomplexprocess,becauseitrequiresthe mas-teryofskillsrelatedtogrammar,topunctuation,tospelling,and tosentenceconstructionandinvolvestheprocessesofplanning, translating,andrevision.Becauseofitscomplexity,manystudents experienceseriousdifficultiesinwriting(Graham,Early,&Wilcox, 2014).Forthisreasonswritinginstructionhasbecomeapriorityin researchandineducationinrecentyears(cf.Graham&Rijlaarsdam,

2016).

Oneofthemostpowerfulinstructionalmethodsfor develop-ingstudentswritingskillsfromalmostallschoolgradelevelsis theSelf-RegulatedStrategyDevelopment(SRSD)model(Harris&

Graham,2016).SRSD hasbeenproven tobe aneffective

writ-ingapproachbyanumberofstudiesandmeta-analyses(Graham,

Harris,&Chambers, 2016).Importantly,SRSDhasalreadybeen

testedinPortugal,demonstratingtobeaneffectivewriting instruc-tionalmethodinthisculturalcontext(Festasetal.,2015;Limpo&

Alves,2014).SRSDaddressesgeneralwritingandspecific

genre-basedstrategies,self-regulationstrategies,theknowledgeneeded toapplywritingstrategies,andmotivationalaspectssuchas self-efficacyforwritingandattributionstoeffort.

Addingacooperativecomponenttoasuccessfulwriting instruc-tionmethodwillbepromisingsince cooperationisseenasone ofthekeyelementsrequiredtoenhanceeffectivewriting instruc-tion.Ineffect,manystudiesconductedwithstudentsfromdifferent gradelevelsinbothregularandspecialclassroomshaveshownthe benefitsofcooperativewritingsettings(cf.VanSteendam,2016): collaborativewritinghelps16and17yearsoldstudentsto emu-latefromeachother’swritingandtolearnphilosophicalconcepts

(Corcelles&Castelló,2015);adultSecondLanguagestudents

pro-ducedmoreaccuratetexts whentheywriteinpairsthanwhen theywriteindividually(Storch,2005);learningdisabled4th-,5th-, and6th-gradestudentsworkinginpairsmademorerevisionsand wrotebettertextsthancontrolgroupstudentswritingwithoutpeer support(MacArthur,Schwartz,&Graham,1991).

Beingincludedinabroaderwritingresearch(formoredetails

seePrata,deSousa,Festas,&Oliveira,2018),thepresentstudyis

aimedtoknowwhetheracooperativemethodsuchasjigsaw, cou-pledwithaninstructionalwritingprogramsuchastheSRSDmodel

(Harris&Graham,2017;Harris, Graham,Mason, &Friedlander,

2008),wouldinfluencestudents’engagementinschool.While pre-viousresearchhasdemonstratedtheroleofmotivationonwriting

(MacArthur&Graham,2016), only fewstudies have been

con-ductedabouttherelationshipbetweenstudentengagementand writing(cf.Boscolo&Hidi,2007).Thepresentstudywasdesigned toaddressthisissue.Furthermore,byaddingacooperativemethod toaninstructionalwritingprogram,itwouldbepossibletobetter understandtherelationshipbetweenwriting,studentengagement andcooperativemethods.

As cooperative methods are a powerful tool for enhancing academic writing proficiency, and given the assumption that engagementshouldbeinfluencedbysuchproficiency,we antic-ipatedthatstudentsintheexperimentalgroupwould,attheend oftheexperiment,bemoreengagedinschoolandachievebetter resultsonStudents’EngagementinSchool:FourdimensionalScale (SES-4DS).Weexpectedbetterresultsintotalscoresandineach ofthedimensionsaddressedbythequestionnaire(cognitive, affec-tive,behavioralandagency).Thus,becauseexperimentalstudents participated ina cooperative setting wheretheycouldincrease theirwritingcompetencetoalevelnecessarytomaintain moti-vationtoengageinschoolactivities,weexpectedtofindstudents moreengagedinschool.

Method

Participants

Thisstudywascarriedoutinthreemiddleschoolsrandomly chosenbetweenallpublicmiddleschoolsofaPortuguesecity.In thecontinuityofpreviousresearch,ourstudywasconductedonly onpublicschools(Festasetal.,2015).Becauseresearchteamsin Portugalarenotallowedtodrawstudentsfromintactclasses,and sincethepopulationsofSchools1and2togetherwerealmostas largeasthepopulationofSchool3,twogroupsofschools were formed(Schools1and2formedonegroupandSchool3another group). These two groups werethen randomlyassigned to the experimental(Schools1and2)andthecontrol(School3)groups.

Beforethestudybegan, thenecessaryconsentwasobtained from the Portuguese Ministry of Education, from the Director of each school, from the students’ parents, from the teachers andfromthestudentsthemselves.Sixninth-gradeclasses from Schools1and2,with60and82students,respectively(thatformed

(4)

theexperimentalgroup)andsixninth-gradeclassesfromSchool 3with135students(whichformedthecontrolgroup)tookpart inthisstudy.Ninthgradewasselectedasthetargetgradelevel inresponsetoschools’andteachers’request.Infact,theapplied writinginstructionshouldhelpthesestudentstoprepareforthe nationalexamthatistakenintheninthgrade.

Consentfromparentstoparticipatewasobtainedforall stu-dentsoftheexperimentalgroup,butnotforeightstudentsinthe controlgroup.Afterexcludingthosestudentswhoreceivespecial educationservicesandthosemissingthepre-testorthepost-test, atotalof213studentswereadmittedtothestudy(113 belong-ing to the experimental group and 100to the control group). Thepercentageofmortalitywas20.4%intheexperimentalgroup and 25.9% in thecontrol group. Althoughthe sample couldbe larger,itincludedanumberofparticipantsverysimilartothoseof otherinstructionalprogramsresearch(e.g.,Cejudo,Salido-López,

&Rodrigo-Ruiz,2017;DeLaPaz&Graham,2002;Harris,Graham,

&Mason,2006).AllstudentsinthestudywerePortuguesenative

speakers.

Concerningthesampledistributionbysex,fromthe113 exper-imentalgroupstudents,68wereboys(60.2%)and45girls(39.8%), andfromthe100controlgroupstudents,43wereboys(43%)and 57 girls(57%). The proportion of maleswas statistically differ-entbetweenthecontrol andtheexperimentalgroups (p=.020; females:p=.235),withahigherpercentageofboysinthe experi-mentalgroup.Theageoftheparticipantsintheexperimentalgroup rangedfrom14to17yearsold(M=14.71;SD=.84;n=113)and theageof thecontrolstudentsrangedfrom14 to16years old (M=14.39;SD=.43;n=100).Theretentionofahighnumberof stu-dentsexplainedthedifferenceofageswithinthesameclass.Thisis particularlytruefortheexperimentalgroupthatheld31ofthe35 retentions.Anindependentt-testrevealedastatisticallysignificant differenceinaget(211)=3.41,p=.001,withexperimentalstudents olderthancontrolones.

Comparingtheeducationallevelcompletedbystudents’parents (rangingfromlessthancompulsoryeducationtoadoctoraldegree), statistical differences were found for fathers (Mann–Whitney U=5.632, p<.001) and for mothers (Mann–Whitney U=6.170, p<.001)betweenthecontrolandtheexperimentalgroups.Inboth casesthecontrolgroupshowedhigherlevelsofparents’ educa-tionalbackground.Examiningthenumberofretentions(0,1or 2)of students pergrade, thetwo groups diddifferstatistically (Mann–WhitneyU=−4.635,p<.001),withtheexperimentalgroup displayingahighernumberofstudentretentions.Theevaluation ofthestructuralelementsofstudents’argumentativewritingdid notrevealanystatisticaldifferencebetweenbothconditionsthe experimentalandthecontrolt(194.66)=−1.703,p=.090,d=−0.11

Concerning total engagement, the results between the two groups(experimentalandcontrol)alsorevealednosignificant dif-ferencet(208.69)=.270,p=.787,d=0.003.

Seventeachersagreedtoparticipate:threeintheexperimental andfourinthecontrolgroup.Alltheteacherswerefemale.They allheldteachingcredentialsineducation,andallofthemhad com-pletedanundergraduatedegree.Allteacherstaughtlanguagearts classes.Theteachers’professionalexperiencerangedfrom28to38 yearsintheexperimentalgroup(M=31.67;SD=5.51)andfrom24 to34yearsinthecontrolgroup(M=29.00;SD=4.40).No statis-ticallysignificantdifferenceswerefoundbetweenthetwogroups (Mann–WhitneyU=8.0,p=.629).

Instruments

TheStudents’EngagementinSchool:FourDimensionalScale

(SES-4DS) (Veiga, 2016b)includes a set of 20 statements aimed to

assessstudentengagementinschoolthroughthecognitive, affec-tive, behavioral,and personalagency dimensions. Data obtained

fromtheexploratoryandconfirmatoryfactoranalysis,inthe orig-inalstudywithSES-4DS,suggestedthat thefourdimensionsof studentengagementinschoolarerelativelyindependentofone anotherandthatSES-4DSallowsamultidimensionalmeasurement ofstudentengagementinschool(Veiga,2013,2016b).

Thecognitivedimension(items1–5)coversstudents’personal investmentinlearningapproachesandinself-regulatory strate-gies.Itincludesitemsfocusingontheacademicareasofwriting (e.g.,“WhenIwritemyacademicwork,firstImakeaplanofthe text”),readingcomprehension(e.g.,“WhenIamreading,Itryto understandtheauthor’sintention”),andstudystrategies(e.g.,“I regularlyrevisemynotes,evenwhenIdonothaveexams”).The affectivedimension(items6–10)includesitemsdealingwiththe senseofbelongingtotheschool(e.g.,reverseditem,“Myschool isaplacewhereIfeelexcluded”),andwithemotionsgenerated byschoolandcolleagues(e.g.,“MyschoolisaplacewhereImake friendseasily”).Thebehavioraldimensionitems(11–15)relateto actionsandpracticesdirectedtowardschool(e.g.,reverseditem, “Iamabsentfromschoolwithoutavalidreason”;“Iintentionally disturbtheclass”).Thepersonalagencydimensionitems(16–20) evaluateastudent’sconstructivecontributiontothecourseofthe instructiontheyreceive(e.g.,“Inclass,Iasktheteachersquestions”; “Igivesuggestionstoteacherstoenhanceclasses”).

TheparticipantsrespondtotheinstrumentonaLikert-type 6-pointscale, where1corresponds tototal disagreementand6to totalagreement.Totalengagementscoresrangedfrom55to114, andinternalconsistency,measuredbyCronbach’s␣,was.80.The cognitivedimensionscoresrangedfrom9to27,andinternal consis-tency,measuredbyCronbach’s␣,was.67.Theaffectivedimension scoresrangedfrom12to30,andinternalconsistency,measured byCronbach’s␣,was.78.Thebehavioraldimensionscoresranged from10to30,andinternalconsistency,measuredbyCronbach’s␣, was.89. Theagency dimensionscoresrangedfrom5to30,and internalconsistency,measuredbyCronbach’s␣,was.87.Other elementsrelated tothe instrumentsusedin this study arethe following:cognitive,CR=.78,AVE=.41,McDonald’s(omega)=.68; affective,CR=.87,AVE=.58,McDonald’s=.78;behavioral,CR=.92, AVE=.69, McDonald’s=.91; and agency, CR=.87, AVE=.58 and McDonald’s=.87.Allvalues foundareverysimilartothose pre-sentedbytheauthorofthescalewhenitwasbuilt(Veiga,2016b). Baselineand post-intervention writing probes were usedto assessstudents’argumentativewritingskills.Asusualin argumen-tativewritingresearch,inordertoevaluatewritingcompositions, eachtextwasscoredforthenumberofstructuralelements: intro-ductiontothetopic;takingaside;presentationofthearguments thatsupporttheopposingposition;argumentationfortheposition taken;andconclusion(e.g.,Festasetal.,2015;Ray,Graham,&Liu, 2018).Theevaluationresultedinthetotalscore,whichincludes thepresenceorabsenceofargumentativestructuralelements.For theintroductiontothetopic,takingasideandtheconclusion,“1” wasallocatediftheelementwaspresentinthetextand“0”ifthe elementwasabsent.Thevalueof“1”wasawardedfortheinclusion ofeachargumentandeachcounter-argument.Twograduate stu-dentsindependentlyevaluatedandscoredallpapers.Inter-rater reliability(weightedCohen’sKappa)forthetwoevaluations (pre-testandpost-test)regardingstructuralelementswas,respectively, .95and.98.

Procedure

AllstudentsansweredtheStudents’EngagementinSchool:Four DimensionalScale(SES-4DS),bothbeforeandafterthetraining ses-sions.Atthesamemoments,studentsalsowroteanargumentative composition,inresponsetotwopromptsonthethemeof adoles-centgroups(cf.Prataetal.,2018).Thedatawerecollectedwithin

(5)

theclassroomcontext,incompliancewithallethicalprocedures requiredofthistypeofresearch.

Trainingsessions-Beforebeginningtheclassroominstruction allteachersparticipatedinapractice-basedprofessional develop-mentsessions,andreceivedguidelinesandmaterialsneededto implementthetrainingprogramintheirclassrooms(Ball&Cohen,

1999;Festas etal.,2015; Harriset al.,2012;Harris, Graham,&

Atkins,2015;McKeownetal.,2016;cf.alsoPrataetal.,2018).The

trainingprogram includedtensessions.Thenumberofsessions respectedtheusualdurationofwritinginterventions(forexample,

Festasetal.,2015;DeLaPaz&Graham,2002;Laneetal.,2008),as

wellasthedurationofotherschoolinterventionprogramsaimedat improvingtheadolescentsgeneralproficiency(seeRojas-Andrade,

Leiva-Bahamondes,Vargas,&Squicciarini-Navarro,2017).

After the pre-test, teachers delivered to all students (from thecontrolandfromtheexperimentalgroups)SRSDinstruction overa5-weekperiod(sessions1–5).Duringthisperiodstudents learnedself-regulationstrategies –goal-setting,self-instruction, self-reinforcement, self-monitoring and self-assessment – and writingstrategiesaimedtoplanandtowriteargumentation(Prata

etal.,2018).

Intheexperimentalgroup,studentswereexposedtoa collab-orativemethodandtheyworkedinthecontextofthejigsawover fiveweeklyclasses(sessions6–10).Therefore,teachersdivided stu-dentsintogroupsofthreeorfourmemberseachone–thebase groups.Acommonlearningtask–thedevelopmentofarguments infavorandagainstacontroversialtopic–wasdividedinto equia-lentparts,and eachgroupmemberreceivedoneofthem. After that,teachersformedtheexpertgroupsbringingtogetherthose membersofthebasegroupswhosharedthesamepartofthe com-montask.Theneachgroupofexpertsreadandexploredtextson theirpartofthecommontaskinordertobroadentheir knowl-edgeondifferentpointsofviewforandagainstthetopicunder discussion.Finally,studentsreturnedtotheirbasegroupstoshare informationandtoworktogether,involvingthemselvesin dialog-icalargumentations,aimingtogeneratemorediversearguments, untileachstudentwasabletoindividuallywriteanargumentative

text(cf.Prataetal.,2018).Overfiveweeklylessons(sessions6–10)

thestudentsofthecontrolgroupworkedindividuallywithsimilar supportmaterialstoperformtheargumentativewriting.

Dataanalyses

Fortestingthehypothesesunderstudyamixed-designanalysis ofvariancemodel(alsoknownasasplit-plotANOVA)wasused. Thisstatisticaltest isrecommendedtotestdifferences between twoormoreindependentgroupswhoseparticipantsaresubjected to repeatedmeasures. In our study this test allows us to ver-ifyiftheexperimentalconditionmadesignificantgainsinschool engagementincomparisonwiththegainsofthecontrolgroup. Furthermorethe split-plotANOVA hastheadvantage of allow-ingtheresearcherstocontrolfordifferencesbetweenthegroups characteristicsbeforerunninganyintervention.Alldescriptiveand inferentialstatisticalanalysesinthisstudywereperformedwith IBMSPSSStatistics(version22.0)andthesignificancelevelof5% (␣=.05)wasconsidered.

Results

Outcomemeasures

Answeringtoourresearchquestionsandhypotheses,several mixed between-withinsubjectsanalysesof variance were con-ducted to explore the impact of the intervention program on

Table1

PerformanceofstudentsintheScaleofStudents’EngagementinSchool,considering totalscoresandthescoresobtainedineachofthefourdimensions

Experimentalgroup(n=113) Controlgroup(n=100)

Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test

M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) Total 87.81(12.18) 88.52(10.08) 87.41(9.68) 85.26(10.11) Cognitive 20.01(3.71) 19.29(3.46) 18.30(3.98) 17.47(4.17) Affective 24.25(4.31) 25.23(4.61) 25.93(3.92) 25.16(4.27) Behavioral 24.72(5.79) 27.34(2.61) 26.79(2.61) 26.54(2.58) Agency 18.83(5.45) 16.66(5.09) 16.38(4.98) 16.09(4.82)

studentengagementinschool,measuredatTime1(priortothe intervention)andTime2(aftertheconclusionoftheintervention). A significantinteraction effectwasfound betweentimeand groupwhenconsideringwriting,evaluatedbyargumentative struc-turalelementsF(1,209)=94.24,p<.001,andcontrolledbyageand sex.Theeffectsizeoftheinteractioneffectwas.311(␩p2).The resultsrevealedthattheexperimentalstudentsmadesubstantial gainsinquantitativeevaluation,astheyincludedmorearguments infavorandagainstapositionaboutthetopicindiscussion,and theywrotemoreelementslikeintroductiontothetopic,takinga sideandaconclusion(formoredetailsseePrataetal.,2018).The meansandstandarddeviationsofstudentsintheStudents’ Engage-mentinSchool:FourDimensionalScale(SES-4DS)(Veiga,2016b)are presentedinTable1.

BeforeapplyingtheANOVAtests,weverifytheassumptionsof normality(covariancematricesforthedependentvariablesequal acrossgroups)andofsphericity(equalityofthevariancesofthe dif-ferencesbetweenallpossiblepairsofwithin-subjectconditions) throughBox’sMtestandMauchly’stest,respectively.Bothtests foundnoviolationoftheseassumptions.Theresultsofthemain hypothesisunder investigationarepresentedin Table2.Ascan beseen,nosignificantinteractioneffectwasfoundbetweentime and groupforthetotal engagementscore(SES) F(1,209)=2.548, p=.112,␩p2=.012,controlledforageandsex.Thatmeansthatthe students’engagementasawholedidnotdifferbetween exper-imentalandcontrolgroupsaftertheintervention.Nosignificant effectwasfoundfortimeF(1,209)=.734,p=.392,␩p2=.004,nor asignificantmaineffectwasobtainedforgroupF(1,209)=3.846, p=.051,␩p2=.018.Anidenticalresultwasobtainedforthe cog-nitive dimension:nosignificantinteractioneffect betweentime and groupF(1,209)=.008, p=.927,␩p2=.000 andnosignificant effectfoundfortimeF(1,209)=1.456,p=.229,␩p2=.007.However, forthecognitivedimensionasignificantmaineffectwasobtained forgroupF(1,209)=16.405,p<.001, ␩p2=.073.Asforthe affec-tivedimension,asignificantinteractioneffectwasfoundbetween timeandgroupF(1,209)=6.877,p=.009, ␩p2=.032.Adolescents improvedtheirscoresonaffectivedimensionafterthe interven-tion. No significant effect was found for time F(1, 209)=.023, p=.879,␩p2=.000,norasignificantmaineffectwasobtainedfor groupF(1,209)=1.561,p=.213,␩p2=.007. Regardingthe behav-ioral dimension, a significant interaction effect was also found betweentimeandgroupF(1,209)=11.623,p=.001,␩p2=.053.After theintervention,theexperimentalgroupincreasedtheirscoreson behavioraldimension.NosignificanteffectwasfoundfortimeF(1, 209)=2.927,p=.089,␩p2=.014norasignificantmaineffectwas obtainedforgroupF(1,209)=.052,p=.820,␩p2=.000.Lastly,in ref-erencetotheagencydimension,asignificantinteractioneffectwas alsoobtainedbetweentimeandgroupF(1,209)=5.762,p=.017, ␩p2=.027.Unexpectedly,post-testanalysisindicatedlowerscores ontheexperimentalgroup.Nosignificanteffectwasfoundfortime F(1,209)=.358,p=.550,␩p2=.002,butasignificantmaineffectwas obtainedforgroupF(1,209)=4.467,p=.036,␩p2=.021.

(6)

Table2

Resultsofstudents’engagementinschoolusingmixedbetween-withinsubjectsanova

Source SumofSquares dfa MeanSquare F p Partial2

Total

Between-subjectseffects 650.521 1 650.521 3.846 .051 .018

Subjectsbetween(error) 35350.810 209 169.143

Within-SubjectsEffects 38.13 1 38.13 .734 .392 .004

Time*EGCGb 132.304 1 132.304 2.548 .112 .012

EGCGxSubjectswithintime(error) 10851.490 209.000 51.921 Cognitive

Between-subjectseffects 378.280 1 378.280 16.405 .000 .073

Subjectsbetween(error) 4819.324 209 23.059

Within-SubjectsEffects 9.013 1 9.013 1.456 .229 .007

Time*EGCG .052 1 .052 .008 .927 .000

EGCG×Subjectswithintime(error) 1293.391 209 6.188 Affective

Between-subjectseffects 40.239 1 40.239 1.561 .213 .007

Subjectsbetween(error) 5388.900 209 25.784

Within-SubjectsEffects .257 1 .257 .023 .879 .000

Time*EGCG 75.910 1 75.910 6.877 .009 .032

EGCG×Subjectswithintime(error) 2306.948 209 11.038 Behavioral

Between-subjectseffects .736 1 .736 .052 .820 .000

Subjectsbetween(error) 2971.396 209 14.217

Within-SubjectsEffects 33.819 1 33.819 2.927 .089 .014

Time*EGCG 134.273 1 134.273 11.623 .001 .053

EGCG×Subjectswithintime(error) 2414.482 209 11.553 Agency

Between-subjectseffects 175.754 1 175.754 4.467 .036 .021

Subjectsbetween(error) 8223.043 209 39.345

Within-SubjectsEffects 4.559 1 4.559 .358 .550 .002

Time*EGCG 73.426 1 73.426 5.762 .017 .027

EGCG×Subjectswithintime(error) 2663.511 209 12.744

adf:degreeoffreedom.

b EGCG:ExperimentalGroupControlGroup.

Discussion

Engagementisinfluencedbyacademicproficiency(

Miranda-Zapata et al., 2018; Wonglorsaichon et al., 2014), and by

collaborativeinstructionalapproaches(Wentzel,2009).Being writ-inga crucial academicarea, it seemsreasonabletoexpect that goodcollaborativepracticesaimedtodevelopwritingskillswould increasestudentengagement.Aswehavehypothesized,alongwith gainsinstudents’ argumentativewriting(cf.Prataetal., 2018), studentswholearnedSRSDcombinedwiththejigsaw–the exper-imentalgroup–showedchangesinschoolengagement,although notinallofitsdimensionsorinengagementasawhole.

Theexperimentalstudentsinthepresentstudyshoweda signif-icantincreaseinengagementresultsintheaffectiveandbehavioral dimensions. The jigsaw, a method based on peerrelationships andinvolvingstudentsindialogicalargumentationanddiscussion inordertoproduceargumentativetexts,hadinfluencedstudent engagement, through affective and behavior engagement. That meansthat,concerningtheaffectivedimension,experimental stu-dentsexpressedagreatersenseofidentificationandofbelonging toschoolandshowedmorepositive emotionsrelatedtoschool andcolleaguesthantheircolleaguesfromthecontrolgroup.The effectsizeobtainedpointstoalowmediumchange(Pallant,2010). Experimental students also improved their resultsin terms of engagementinthebehavioraldimensionwiththehighesteffect sizeofengagementinthisstudy.Ineffect,whencomparedwith controlstudents,theexperimentalgroupshowedbetterresultson theSES-4DS’itemsthataimedtoevaluatethebehavioral dimen-sioninaspectssuchasattendanceinclass,attentionduringlessons, andabsenceofdisruptiveconduct.

Our study showed that improving academic proficiency in areassuchaswriting throughefficientmethods suchasjigsaw

can influence engagement, a finding already made by others

(Irvinetal.,2007;Wentzel,2009).However,onlytwodimensions

of engagement–affective and behavioral –increased withour instructional design. The cognitive engagement dimension did not sufferanyinfluence fromtheapplication ofSRSDwiththe jigsaw,andunexpectedly,theagencydimensionevendecreased. Total engagement, not showing a significantinteraction effect, revealedaloweffectsize,whichmeansthatit wasonlyalittle bitaffected by the intervention, which combinedSRSD with a cooperativemethod.Theabsenceofasignificantincreaseintotal engagementmightbeattributabletopartialresultsrelatedtothe lackofincreaseinthecognitiveandagencydimensions.

In analyzingthedata, weobserved thatthere wasa signifi-cantmaineffectofgrouponcognitiveandonagencydomains, asexperimentalstudents had betterresultsthan controlgroup studentsinthesetwodimensions.Thissuperioritymayhavehad some influence onthe absence of results in the cognitive and agency dimensions. In effect, variations acrossthe adolescence weredescribedindifferentadolescentdomainsasscholar

adjust-ment(Veiga,García,Reeve,Wentzel,&García,2015)orpersonal

competence(Riquelme,Garcia,&Serra,2018).Previousworks ana-lyzingmultidimensionalengagementshowedthat,inthemiddle adolescence,themostcompetentstudents(thosewithhigh self-concept) could lose their cognitive engagement and also their personalagencyengagement(Veigaetal.,2015).

In thesame manner,controland experimentalgroups were not similar in certain characteristics such as sex (the experi-mentalgrouphad moreboysthan thecontrolgroup),mothers’ andfathers’educationallevels(thecontrolgroupshowedhigher levelsof parental educationalbackground), and retentions (the experimentalgrouphadahighernumberofstudentretentions). Theexperimentalgroup’sadvantageinthecognitiveandagency

(7)

dimensionsand/orthedifferencesbetweenstudentsofthetwo groupsmaywellhavehadsomeinfluenceontheabsenceofany effectoftreatmentoncognitiveandagencyengagement.AsVan

Steendam(2016)states,itisessentialtoconsidererthe

interac-tionbetweencollaborative,individual,andcontextualvariablesin ordertoanalyzetheeffectsofcooperativemethods.Thisauthor expressedthe need to consider individual characteristics, such asability,writingbeliefsorotherswithrespecttotheoutcomes obtainedwhenapplyingcooperativemethodsinwriting

instruc-tion (Van Steendam, 2016). Such individual characteristics, in

interactionwithcontextualones,mayindeedhavesometypeof influenceonresults.Futureresearchisneededtohighlightsuch interactions,namelyhowsocialandacademicvariablesand pre-viousengagementlevelswouldinteractwithtaskcharacteristics, peerrelationshipanddialogicinvolvement.

Finally,itispossiblethatourresultscanbeexplainedbythe lack of effect of cooperative methods oncognitive and agency engagement.Herrman(2013)showedthatalthoughcooperative learningcouldinfluencesomeaspectsofengagementintermsof actionsandbehaviors,itdidnotimpactoncognitiveengagement activity, or on deep approaches to learning and to schooling. Muchmoreresearchisneededinordertobetterunderstandthe impactofcooperativemethods oneach oneof theengagement dimensions. Although these explanations can be plausible, a deeperunderstandingofthereasonsthatcausedtheworstresults forexperimentalstudentsisneeded,especiallyinrespecttothe decreaseoftheagencydimension.AnalyzingStudents’Engagement inSchool:FourdimensionalScale(SES-4DS)(Veiga,2016b),wecan seethatalmostallitemsaimedatevaluatingtheagencydimension arecenteredonstudents’actionstowardteachers(e.g.,“Inclass, Iaskedtheteachersquestions”).Inourstudyweusedthejigsaw, a cooperative method centered on peer interaction. Students developed theirargumentative writing skillsthrough dialogical argumentationanddiscussionswiththeircolleaguesinthe con-textofthejigsaw(basegroup)andexpertgroups.Perhapssuch amethod,centeredonstudentworkandtheinteractionbetween studentsasopposedtotherelationshipbetweenteacherand stu-dents,hasalesserimpactonstudents’actionstowardtheteacher. Inthefuture,itwillbenecessarytodeepenourknowledgeonthis topicbycontinuingresearchonthisquestionasawaytoascertain how cooperative methods influence the relationship between studentsandteachers.Similarly,itwillbeimportanttoevaluate otheraspectsoftheagencydimension,todeterminewhetherthe negativeeffectin thepresentstudyremains. Inaddition tothe reasonspreviouslynotedfortheresultsobtained,otherscanexist. Agreatersensitivityofthequestionnaireitemsonbehaviorand affectivedimensionsmayexplainthedifferencesfoundinthose dimensionsasopposedtothecognitiveandagencyones.Similarly, althoughinthecognitivedimensiontherewereitemsdealingwith writing,theStudents’EngagementinSchool:FourDimensionalScale (SES-4DS)(Veiga,2016b)isdirectedtoward schoolengagement andnottowritingengagement.Inthefutureitwillbeofinterest todeterminewhetheranintervention thatismorecentered on thecontentsrelatedtothoseincludedinthequestionnaireitems mighthavea greaterimpactonresults,oralternatively,ifusing aquestionnairedirectedmorespecificallytowritingengagement willproduceresultsdifferentfromthoseobtainedinthepresent study.Thepossibilitythatalongertimeinterventioncould pro-duce stronger effects on the cognitive and agency dimensions shouldalsobeexamined.Inthesamemanner,inordertodeepen ourunderstandingabouttheimpactofcooperativemethods on engagement,futureresearchshouldincludeamaintenanceprobe. EventhoughthestudentshaveansweredtheStudents’Engagement inSchool:FourDimensionalScale(SES-4DS)(Veiga,2016b), both beforeandafterthetrainingsessions,itwouldhavebeenimportant thattheyalsoansweredatathirdmoment,oneortwomonthsafter

theintervention.Becauseourresourceswerelimitedwelacked thefundstoapplyafollow-uptest.Futureresearchonthe mainte-nanceofeffectsofthecooperativewritinginstructionalprogramon studentengagementshouldincludeathirdassessmentmoment.

Tosummarize,wecanconcludethatusingcooperative meth-odssuchasjigsawinareasasimportantforacademicproficiency aswritingcouldinfluencecertainaspectsofengagement,more pre-cisely,thefieldsofbehavioralandaffectiveengagement.However, thepresentstudyraisesmanydoubtsabouttheeffectsofthistype ofmethodonotheraspectsofengagement,namelythosewhich relatetothecognitiveandagencydimensions.Indeed,although experimentalstudentsincreasedtheirlevelsofaffectiveand behav-ioralengagementaftertheintervention,theydidnotincreasetheir cognitiveandagencyengagement,obtainingevenworseresults thancontrolstudentsinthelatterdimension.Asotherresearchers havedefended,itisessentialtobroadenthepresentresearchin ordertobetterunderstandtheinteractions betweenindividual, cooperativeandcontextualvariables(VanSteendam,2016).Itis alsoessentialtoascertaintheextenttowhichcooperativelearning mightaffectthoseaspectsofengagementmorerelatedtocognitive processing,andtostudents’constructivecontributiontothecourse oftheinstructiontheyreceive,i.e.,agencyengagement(Herrman, 2013).Inthesamemanner,itwillbeafutureinteresttoplantrue experimentaldesigns and interventions, includingmaintenance probes.Thesetrueexperimentaldesignsshouldhavecontentsand measureinstrumentsthatarebetteradjustedtoeachother,and theyshouldtakeplaceoveralongerperiodoftime.

References

Ainley, M. D. (1993). Styles of engagement with learning: Multidi-mensional assessment of their relationship with strategy use and school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(3), 395–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.395

Appleton,J.,Christenson,S.,&Furlong,M.(2008).Studentengagementwithschool: Criticalconceptualandmethodologicalissuesoftheconstruct.Psychologyinthe Schools,45(5),369–386.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.20303/Abstract Aronson,E.,Stephan,C.,Sikes,J.,Blaney,N.,&Snapp,M.(1978).Thejigsawclassroom.

BeverlyHills,CA:SagePublications,Inc.

Ball,D.L.,&Cohen,D.K.(1999).Developingpracticedevelopingpractitioners: Toward a practice-based theory of professional education. In L. Darling-Hammond,&G.Sykes(Eds.),Teachingasalearningprofession:Handbookfor policyandpractice(pp.3–31).SanFrancisco,CA:Jossey-Bass.

Bommarito, D. V. (2015). Collaborative research writing as mentoring in a U.S. English doctoralprogram. Journalof WritingResearch, 8(2), 267–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.17239/jowr-2016.08.02.04

Boscolo,P.,&Hidi,S.(2007).Themultiplemeaningsofmotivationtowrite.InS.Hidi, &P.Boscolo(Eds.),Writingandmotivation(pp.1–14).Amsterdam:Elsevier. Buijs,M.,&Admiraal,W.(2013).Homeworkassignmentsto enhancestudent

engagementinsecondaryeducation.EuropeanJournalofPsychologyofEducation, 28(3),767–779.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0139-0

Carrasco, C., Alarcón, R., & Trianes, M. V. (2018). Adaptación y trabajo cooperativo en el alumnado de educación primaria desde la percep-ción del profesorado y la familia. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 23(1), 56–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psicod.2017.02.001

Cejudo, J., Salido-López, J. V., & Rodrigo-Ruiz, D. (2017). Efecto de un pro-grama para la mejora en competencia en comunicación lingüística de alumnadodeEducaciónSecundaria.RevistadePsicodidáctica,22(2),135–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psicod.2016.11.001

Corcelles,S.M.,&Castelló,M.(2015).Learningphilosophicalthinkingthrough col-laborativewritinginsecondaryeducation.JournalofWritingResearch,7(1), 157–200.http://dx.doi.org/10.17239/jowr-2015.07.01.07

DeLaPaz,S.,&Graham,S.(2002).Explicitlyteachingstrategies,skills,and knowl-edge:Writinginstructioninmiddleschoolclassrooms.JournalofEducational Psychology,94(4),687–698.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.4.687 Festas,I.,Oliveira,A.L.,Rebelo,J.A.,Damião,H.,Harris,K.R.,&Graham,S.(2015).

Pro-fessionaldevelopmentinSelf-RegulatedStrategyDevelopment:Effectsonthe writingperformanceofeighthgradePortuguesestudents.Contemporary Educa-tionalPsychology,40,17–27.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.05.004 Fidalgo,R.,Harris,K.R.,&Braaksma,M.(2017).Designprinciplesforteaching effec-tivewriting:Anintroduction.InR.Fidalgo,K.R.Harris,&M.Braaksma(Eds.), Studiesinwritingseries:Designprinciplesforteachingeffectivewriting(34)(pp. 3–12).Leiden:Brill.

Fredricks,J.A.,Blumenfeld,P.C.,&Paris,A.H.(2004).Schoolengagement: Poten-tialoftheconcept,stateoftheevidence.ReviewofEducationalResearch,74(1), 59–109.http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543074001059

(8)

Fuentes,M.C.,García,F.,Gracia,E.,&Alarcón,A.(2015).Parentalsocializationstyles andpsychologicaladjustment.AstudyinSpanishadolescents.Revistade Psico-didáctica,20(1),117–138.http://dx.doi.org/10.1387/RevPsicodidact.10876 Gillies,R.M.(2007).Cooperativelearning:Integratingtheoryandpractice.Thousand

Oaks,CA:Sage.

Gillies,R.M.(2017).Promotingacademicallyproductivestudentdialogue dur-ingcollaborativelearning.InternationalJournalofEducationalResearch,1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2017.07.014

Graham,S.,Early,J.,&Wilcox,K.(2014).Adolescentwritingandwriting instruc-tion:Introductiontothespecialissue.ReadingandWriting:AnInterdisciplinary Journal,27(6),969–972.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9497-0 Graham,S.,Harris,K.,&Chambers,A.(2016).Evidence-basedpracticeandwriting

instruction:Areviewofreviews.InC.MacArthur,S.Graham,&J.Fitzgerald(Eds.), Handbookofwritingresearch(2nded.,34,pp.211–226).NewYork:Guilford Press.

Graham,S.,&Rijlaarsdam,G.(2016).Writingeducationaroundtheglobe: Intro-ductionandcallforanewglobalanalysis.ReadingandWriting,29(5),781–792. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9640-1

Gutiérrez-Fresneda, R. (2018). Longitudinal study on the development of literacy skills during literacy. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 23(2), 137–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psicod.2017.09.002

Gutiérrez,M.,Tomás,J.M.,Romero,I.,&Barrica,J.M.(2017).Perceivedsocialsupport, schoolengagementandsatisfactionwithschool.RevistadePsicodidactica,22(2), 111–117.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psicod.2017.01.001

Hagenauer,G.,Hascher,T.,&Volet,S.(2015).Teacheremotionsintheclassroom: Associationswithstudents’engagement,classroomdiscipline,andthe interper-sonalteacher-studentrelationship.EuropeanJournalofPsychologyofEducation, 30(4),385–403.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-015-0250-0

Harris,K.R.,&Graham,S.(2016).Self-RegulatedStrategyDevelopmentin writ-ing:Policyimplicationsofanevidence-basedpractice.Reading,Writing,and Language,3(1),7–84.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2372732215624216 Harris,K.R.,&Graham,S.(2017).Self-RegulatedStrategyDevelopment:Theoretical

bases,criticalinstructionalelementsandfutureresearch.InR.Fidalgo,K.R. Har-ris,&M.Braaksma(Eds.),Studiesinwritingseries:Designprinciplesforteaching effectivewriting(Vol.34)(pp.119–151).Leiden:Brill.

Harris, K. R., Graham, S., & Adkins, M. (2015). Practice-based professional developmentand Self-RegulatedStrategy Development forTier 2, at-risk writers in second grade. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 40, 5–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.02.003

Harris,K.R.,Graham,S.,&Mason,L.H.(2006).Improvingthewriting,knowledge, andmotivationofstrugglingyoungwriters:EffectsofSelf-RegulatedStrategy Developmentwithandwithoutpeersupport.AmericanEducationalResearch Journal,43(2),295–340.http://aer.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/43/2/295 Harris,K.R.,Graham,S.,Mason,L.,&Friedlander,B.(2008).Powerfulwritingstrategies

forallstudents.Baltimore:PaulH.BrookesPublishingCo.

Harris, K. R., Lane, K. L., Graham, S., Driscoll, S., Sandmel, K., Brindle, M., & Schatschneider, C. (2012). Practice-based professional development for self-regulated strategies development in writing: A random-ized controlled study. Journal of Teacher Education, 63(2), 103–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022487111429005

Herrman,K.(2013).Theimpactofcooperativelearningonstudentengagement: Resultsfromanintervention.ActiveLearninginHigherEducation,14(3),175–187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1469787413498035

Irvin,J.,Meltzer,J.,&Dukes,M.(2007).Takingactiononadolescentliteracy:An imple-mentationguideforschoolleaders.Alexandria,VA:AssociationforSupervision& CurriculumDevelopment.

Jimerson,S.,Campos,E.,&Greif,J.(2003).Towardsanunderstandingofdefinitions andmeasuresofstudentengagementinschoolsandrelatedterms.TheCalifornia SchoolPsychologist,8(1),7–28.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03340893 Johnson,M.K.,Crosnoe,R.,&Elder,G.H.(2001).Students’attachmentand

aca-demicengagement:Theroleofraceandethnicity.SociologyofEducation,74(4), 318–340.http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2673138

Johnson,D.W.,&Johnson,R.T.(1999).Learningtogetherandalone.Cooperative, competitiveandindividuallearning(5thed.).NeedhamHeights,MA:Allynand Bacon.

Johnson,D.W.,&Johnson,R.T.(2009).Aneducationalpsychologysuccessstory: Socialinterdependencetheoryandcooperativelearning.EducationalResearcher, 38(5),365–379.http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0013189X09339057

Johnson,D.W.,&Johnson,R.T.(2016).Cooperativelearningandteaching citizen-shipindemocracies.InternationalJournalofEducationalResearch,76,162–177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2015.11.009

Johnson,D.W.,Johnson,R.T.,&Smith,K.A.(2007).Thestateofcooperative learn-inginpost-secondaryandprofessionalsettings.EducationalPsychologyReview, 19(1),15–29.http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9038-8

Jordan,W.J.,&Nettles,S.M.(2000).Howstudentsinvesttheirtimeoutsideof school:Effectsonschool-relatedoutcomes.SocialPsychologyofEducation,3(4), 217–243.http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:100965561

Klein,P.,Arcon,N.,&Baker,S.(2016).Writingtolearn.InC.MacArthur,S.Graham,& J.Fitzgerald(Eds.),Handbookofwritingresearch(2nded.,Vol.34,pp.243–256). NewYork:TheGuilfordPress.

Klein,P.,Boscolo,P.,Kirkpatrick,L.,&Gelati,C.(Eds.).(2014).Writingasalearning activity.Leiden:Brill.

Kuhn,D.(2015).Thinkingtogetherandalone.EducationalResearcher,44(1),46–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0013189X15569530

Lane, K. L., Harris, K. R., Graham, S., Weisenbach, J. L., Bindle, M., & Morphy, P. (2008). The effects of Self-Regulated Strategy Development

on the writing performance of second-grade Students with behavioral and writing difficulties. The Journal of SpecialEducation, 41(4), 234–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022466907310370

León del Barco, B., Mendo-Lázaro, S., Felipe-Casta ˜no, E., Polo del Río, M. I., & Fajardo-Bullón, F. (2017). Potencia de equipo y aprendizaje coop-erativo en el ámbito universitario. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 22(1), 9–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1387/RevPsicodidact.14213

Limpo,T.,&Alves,R.A.(2014).Implicittheoriesofwritingandtheirimpacton stu-dents’responsetoaSRSDintervention.BritishJournalofEducationalPsychology, 84(4),571–590.http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12042

MacArthur,C.,&Graham,S.(2016).Writingresearchfromacognitiveperspective. InC.MacArthur,S.Graham,&J.Fitzgerald(Eds.),Handbookofwritingresearch (pp.24–40).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

MacArthur,C.A.,Schwartz,S.,&Graham,S.(1991).Effectsofareciprocalpeer revi-sionstrategyinspecialeducationclassrooms.LearningDisabilitiesResearchand Practice,6(4),201–210.

Martínez,I.,Cruise,E.,García,Ó.F.,&Murgui,S.(2017).Englishvalidationofthe ParentalSocializationScale–ESPA29.FrontiersinPsychology,8(865),1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00865

Martínez, I., Murgui, S., García, Ó. F., & Garcia, F. (2019). Parenting in the digital era: Protective and risk parenting styles for traditional bullying andcyberbullyingvictimization. ComputersinHumanBehavior,90, 84–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.036

McKeown, D., Brindle, M., Harris, K. R., Graham, S., Collins, A., & Brown, M. (2016). Illuminating growth and struggles using mixed methods: Practice-based professional development and coaching for differentiat-ing SRSD instruction in writing. Reading and Writing, 29(6), 1105–1140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9627-y

Miranda-Zapata, E., Lara, L., Navarro, J. J., Saracostti, M., & De-Toro, X. (2018). Modeling the effect of school engagement on attendance to classes and school performance.Revista de Psicodidáctica, 23(2), 102–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psicoe.2018.03.001

O’Donnell,A.M.,Hmelo-Silver,C.E.,&Erkens,G.(2013).Collaborativelearning, reasoningandtechnology(1sted.).Hoboken:TaylorandFrancis.

Pallant, J. (2010). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS (4th ed.). Maidenhead: Open University Press/McGraw-Hill.

Patrick,H.,Ryan,A.,&Kaplan,A.(2007).Earlyadolescents’perceptionsofthe class-roomsocialenvironment,motivationalbeliefs,andengagement.Journalof Edu-cationalPsychology,99(1),83–98.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.83 Prata,M.J.,deSousa,B.,Festas,I.,&Oliveira,A.L.(2018).Cooperativemethods andSRSDasatooltoimplementargumentativewritingskills.TheJournalof EducationalResearch,1–16.http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1427037 Ray,A.B.,Graham,S.,&Liu,X.(2018).EffectsofSRSDcollegeentranceessayexam instructionforhighschoolstudentswithdisabilitiesorat-riskforwriting diffi-culties.ReadingandWriting.,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9900-3 Reeve,J.,&Tseng,C.(2011).Agencyasafourthaspectofstudents’engagement

duringlearningactivities.ContemporaryEducationalPsychology,36(4),257–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.05.002

Riquelme,M.,García,Ó.F.,&Serra,E.(2018).Psychosocialmaladjustmentin ado-lescence:Parentingstyles,self-esteemandsubstanceuse.AnalesdePsicología, 34,536–544.http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.34.3.315201

Rodrigues,Y.,Veiga,F.H.,Fuentes,M.C.,&García,F.(2013).Parentingand ado-lescents’self-esteem:ThePortuguesecontext.RevistadePsicodidáctica,18(2), 395–416.http://dx.doi.org/10.1387/RevPsicodidact.6842

Roeser, R., Eccles, J., & Sameroff, A. (2000). School as a context of early adolescents’ academic and social-emotional development: A summary of research findings. The Elementary School Journal, 100(5), 443–471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/499650

Rojas-Andrade,R.,Leiva-Bahamondes,L.,Vargas,A.M.B.,&Squicciarini-Navarro,A. M.(2017).Effectsofimplementationfidelityoftheresultsofapreventive inter-ventioninschoolmentalhealth:Amultilevelanalysis.PsychosocialIntervention, 26(3),147–154.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psi.2016.12.002

Roseth,C.J.,Johnson,D.W.,&Johnson,R.T.(2008).Promotingearlyadolescents’ achievementandpeerrelationships:Theeffectsofcooperative,competitive, and individualistic goalstructures. PsychologicalBulletin, 134(2), 223–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.223

Ryan, A. M., & Patrick, H. (2001). The classroom social environment and changes in adolescents’ motivation and engagement during mid-dle school. American Educational Research Journal, 38(2), 437–460. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00028312038002437

Sharan,S.(1999).Handbookofcooperativelearningmethods.NewYork:Praeger. Simon-Morton, B., & Chen, R. (2009). Peer and parent influences on school

engagement among early adolescents. Youth & Society, 41(1), 3–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0044118X09334861

Slavin,R.(2014).Cooperativelearninginelementaryschools.Education,43(1),5–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03004279.2015.963370

Storch, N. (2005). Collaborative writing: Product, process, and stu-dents’ reflections. Journal of Second Language Writing, 14(3), 153–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jslw.2005.05.002

Todd,R.,&Dadlani,P.(2014).Collaborativeinformationusebyhighschool stu-dentsinadigitallearningenvironment:Connectingmetatheory,theoretical frameworksandmethodology.LibrariesintheDigitalAge(LIDA)Proceedings,13. http://ozk.unizd.hr/proceedings/index.php/lida/article/view/155

VanSteendam,E.(2016).Editorial:Formsofcollaborationinwriting.Journalof WritingResearch,8(2),183–204.http://dx.doi.org/10.17239/jowr-2016.08.02.01

(9)

Vedder, P., & Veendrick, A. M. (2003). Reward structure. Scandinavian Jour-nal of Educational Research, 47(5), 529–542. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 0031383032000122444

Veiga,F.H.(2013).Envolvimentodosalunosnaescola:Elaborac¸ãodeumanova escaladeavaliac¸ão/Students’engagementinschool:Constructionofanew eval-uatingscale.InternationalJournalofDevelopmentalandEducationalPsychology, 1(1),441–450.http://dx.doi.org/10.17979/reipe.2017.0.05.2189

Veiga,F.H.(Coord.).(2016).(E-book)Envolvimentodosalunosnaescola: Perspeti-vasdapsicologiaeeducac¸ão.Motivac¸ãoparaodesempenhoacadémico/Students’ Engagementin School: Perspectives ofpsychology andeducation.Motivation foracademicperformance.Lisboa:InstitutodeEducac¸ãodaUniversidadede Lisboa.

Veiga,F.H.(2016).Assessingstudentengagementinschool:Developmentand vali-dationofafour-dimensionalscale.Procedia-SocialandBehavioralSciences, 813–819.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2016.02.153

Veiga,F.H.,García,F.,Reeve,J.,Wentzel,K.,&García,Ó.F.(2015).Whenadolescents withhighself-conceptlosetheirengagementinschool.RevistadePsicodidáctica, 20(2),305–320.http://dx.doi.org/10.1387/RevPsicodidact.12671

Wentzel,K.R.(2009).Peersandacademicfunctioningatschool.InK.Rubin,W. Bukowski,&B.Laursen(Eds.),Handbookofpeerinteractions,relationships,and groups.Social,emotional,andpersonalitydevelopmentincontext(pp.531–547). NewYork,NY:GuilfordPress.

Wonglorsaichon, B.,Wongwanich, S.,&Wiratchai,N.(2014).TheInfluenceof studentsschoolengagementonlearningachievement:Astructuralequation modelinganalysis.Procedia-SocialandBehavioralSciences,116,1748–1755. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.467

Zumbrunn, S., Marrs, S., & Mewborn, C. (2016). Toward a better under-standing of student perceptions of writing feedback: A mixed methods study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29(2), 349–370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9599-3

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Our observations are contrasting those of MacDonald &amp; Butler (1981) because the ex-type strain (CBS 110.81) did form microconidia; also, sequences of CBS 110.81

Explanations for the currently low application involved three main findings: (1) carmakers lack data curation and exploitation capabilities; (2) carmakers aim to cover the whole

Pode-se, por isso, afirmar que as novas missões assumidas pelos Estados e confiadas às organizações internacionais e aos respectivos aparelhos militares (missões humanitá-

série do ensino médio estão envolvidos em jornada de atividades que não incluem elevado tempo de entretenimento sedentário baseado em tela, como, por exemplo, as atividades físicas

The probability of attending school four our group of interest in this region increased by 6.5 percentage points after the expansion of the Bolsa Família program in 2007 and

Estudos posteriores permitiram chegar uma versão reduzida deste instrumento, constituída por 32 itens que, da mesma forma que a versão original, avalia os três estilos parentais:

Entrou-se num círculo infernal de insatisfações: insatisfação dos professores que não dispõem dos indispensáveis equipamentos e de alunos devidamente socializados e

Esta pesquisa tem como objetivo conhecer a visão dos idosos em relação à Justiça e em relação ao trabalho desenvolvido no Setor de Conciliação ou de Mediação Cível do Fórum