• Nenhum resultado encontrado

A set of 400 pictures standardised for Portuguese: norms for name agreement, familiarity and visual complexity for children and adults

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Share "A set of 400 pictures standardised for Portuguese: norms for name agreement, familiarity and visual complexity for children and adults"

Copied!
8
0
0

Texto

(1)

A SET OF 400 PICTURES STANDARDISED

FOR PORTUGUESE

Norms for name agreement , familiarit y

and visual complexit y for children and adult s

Sabine Pompéia

1

, Mônica Carolina Miranda

1

, Orlando Francisco Amodeo Bueno

2

ABSTRACT - The present article provides normative measures for 400 pictured objects (Cycow icz et al., 1997) view ed by Port uguese speaking Brazilian Universit y st udent s and 5-7 year-old children. Nam e agreem ent , familiarity and visual complexity ratings w ere obtained. These variables have been show n to be important for t he select ion of adequat e st imuli for cognit ive st udies. Children’s name agreement w as low er t han t hat of adult s. The children also failed t o provide adequat e modal names for 103 concept s, rat ed draw ings as less familiar and less complex, and chose short er names for pict ures. The differences in rat ings bet w een adult s and children w ere higher than those observed in the literature employing smaller picture sets. The pattern of correlations among measures observed in the present study w as consistent w ith previous reports, supporting the usefulness of the 400 picture set as a tool for cognitive research in different cultures and ages.

KEY WORDS: picture, naming, familiarity, visual complexity, children, adults.

Conjunto de 400 figuras padronizadas para o português: normas de nomeação, familiaridade e complexidade visual para crianças e adultos

RESUM O - Est e art igo fornece dados normat ivos para o Brasil de um conjunt o de 400 figuras de objet os (Cycow icz et al., 1997) avaliados por estudantes universitários e crianças de 5–7 anos. Foram obtidos dados referent es à consist ência de nomeação, familiaridade com os objet os represent ados e complexidade visual dos desenhos. Existem evidências de que essas variáveis são importantes para a adequada seleção de estímulos para estudos cognitivos. A consistência de nomeação das crianças foi menor que a dos adultos. Em relação aos adult os, as crianças não conseguiram nom ear adequadam ent e 103 conceit os, avaliaram os desenhos como sendo menos familiares e menos complexos e escolheram nomes mais curtos para as figuras. As diferenças nas avaliações entre adultos e crianças foram mais altas que as observadas na literatura que envolveu conjuntos menores de desenhos. O padrão de correlações entre medidas observadas no presente trabalho são consistentes com relat os ant eriores, o que dá suport e à ut ilidade desse conjunt o de 400 figuras como ferrament a para pesquisas cognitivas em diferentes culturas e faixas etárias.

PALAVRAS-CHAVE: figura, nomeação, familiaridade, complexidade visual, crianças, adult os.

Department of Psychobiology, Universidade Federal de São Paulo (UNIFESP), São Paulo SP - Brazil. 1Graduate Student; 2Professor. Financial

Support : AFIP and FAPESP (grant no. 1998/15397-1).

Received 17 Oct ober 2000, received in final form 19 January 2001. Accept ed 26 January 2001.

Orlando F.A. Bueno - Departamento de Psicobiologia-UNIFESP - R. Botucatu 826, 1° andar - 04023-062 São Paulo SP - Brasil. FAX 55 11 5572 5092. E-mail: ofabueno@psicobio.epm.br

Naming object s is a fundament al abilit y t hat hu-mans use t o communicat e t hrough language. The apparent simplicit y of naming belies t he complexit y of its underlying cognitive processes1,2. Investigations

of t he underlying cognit ive processes involved in naming t herefore require t hat st udies be conduct ed under carefully cont rolled condit ions t hat have of-t en included of-t he use of picof-t ured objecof-t s as labora-t ory analogues of objeclabora-t labora-t hem selves. How ever, no normat ive dat a for such st imuli w as available unt il

t he end of t he 1970s, precluding adequat e compari-sons bet w een st udies t hat employed het erogeneous draw ings. A t urning point in t his line of st udy w as t he publicat ion of Snodgrass & Vanderw art ’s3 st

(2)

as size of draw ings (see 4), number of det ails and

orient at ion3.

Among t he most import ant aspect s of pict ure naming are name agreement , or t he rat e at w hich object s depict ed in t he draw ings are referred t o w it h t he same name, familiarit y w it h t he concept s and visual complexit y of t he draw ings. These measures are essent ially independent and may be assumed t o affect different st ages during pict ure processing5.

Name agreement is a robust predict or of naming difficult y and is import ant for st udies of naming la-tency, picture-name matching, recall, recognition and invest igat ions in w hich verbal coding is m anipula-t ed6. Familiarit y is an import ant predict or of pict ure

nam ing lat encies (t he m ore f am iliar t he concept s, t he short er t he naming t ime), w hile visual complex-it y affect s variables such as naming lat ency, t achis-t oscopic recogniachis-t ion achis-t hreshold and memorabiliachis-t y6.

Since t he pioneering paper of Snodgrass & Van-derw art3 in t he USA, sim ilar w ork has been

con-duct ed on t he 260 pict ure-set for Brit ish7, Spanish8,

Japanese9 and Icelandic populat ions. A prelim inary

adapt at ion int o Port uguese w as also accomplished w it h a smaller (150) set of pict ures11 . A larger set of

400 pictures has been studied more recently for both Nort h Am ericans5 and Frenchmen6. Normat ive dat a

for different languages show t hat despit e pict ures being judged t o be of similar familiarit y and visual complexit y, name agreement is specific t o t he par-ticular language investigated8. Hence, normative data

must be obt ained for every language under w hich research em ploying draw ings are conduct ed. In addit ion t o t he import ance of considering t he na-t ive na-t ongue of na-t he subjecna-t s used in sna-t udies of picna-t ure naming, t he age of part icipant s must also be t aken int o considerat ion. M ost of t he normat ive dat a cit ed above w as obt ained solely for universit y st udent s (> 18 years of age, hereaft er referred t o as adult s). Except ions are t he st udies by Berman et al.12 and

Cycow icz et al.5, w ho employed 7 t o 10, and 5 t o 7

year-olds, respect ively. Norms for t he younger chil-dren w ere found t o be different from t hose of bot h older children and adult s, suggest ing t hat t hey ex-hibit immat ure lexical and/or semant ic net w orks5.

Normat ive dat a in ot her languages must t herefore also include subject s of dif f erent ages in order t o assure that age-appropriate pictorial stimuli are avail-able. There is an obvious advant age at obt aining norm s f or sm all children because t he concept s t hat t hey can name correct ly are useful for all ot her ages and can be used in developm ent al st udies. Norm s f or young adult s are also essent ial in all languages because universit y st udent s const it ut e t he m ost

w idely used populat ion in st udies of cognit ive psy-chology.

The present st udy aimed at obt aining normat ive dat a on t he 400 pict ure-set proposed by Cycow icz et al.5 t o be used in Port uguese speaking

popula-t ions from Brazil. Subjecpopula-t s w ere middle-class univer-sit y st udent s and 5 t o 7 year-olds children. Name agreement , pict ure familiarit y, visual complexit y and lengt h of modal names w ere st udied, closely follow -ing Cycow icz et al.’s5 met hods.

METHOD

Subjects

All middle-class, nat ive Port uguese speakers w ho lived in t he cit y of São Paulo, Brazil. Their social-economic st a-t us w as dea-t ermined by a-t he ABIPEM E scale, developed by t he “ Brazilian Associat ion of t he Inst it ut e of M arket Re-search” .

a. Adult s: 150 universit y st udent s (24 m en), aged 23.6± 6.9 years (mean± SD).

b. Children: t hirt y-six children (18 boys), 5 t o 7 year-olds (83.9± 7.4 m ont hs; range 73-95 m ont hs), w ho had normal behaviour as assessed using t he CATRS-1013.

Stimuli

Four-hundred pict ures of common object s draw n in black over w hit e background5. These pict ure originat ed from different sources [pict ureset 1 cont aining 260 draw -ings3; pict ure-set 2 cont aining 61 draw ings12; pict ure-set 3 w it h 79 st ylist ically similar draw ings t o t he ones in t he first 2 set s but obt ained elsew here5]. The pict ures w ere randomly divided int o 5 list s of 80 pict ures. The order of present at ion of t hese list s w as balanced across subject s and sessions.

Procedure

Essent ially t he same as employed by Cycow icz et al.5, except t hat only naming, familiarit y and complexit y w ere det ermined. The et hics commit t ee approved t he prot o-col. Informed assent and consent forms w ere obt ained from t he adult part icipant s, t he children and t heir par-ent s.

(3)

con-sidered DKN (“ doesn’t know name” ) and familiarit y and complexit y rat ings w ere obt ained. If t he child failed t o answ er t he quest ions concerning t he nat ure of t he ob-ject , naming w as scored as DKO (“ doesn’t know obob-ject ” ) and t he next pict ure w as present ed. Pract ice pict ures w ere show n at t he beginning of each session. To illust rat e t he familiarit y rat ings, pict ures of an ice-cream (very familiar) and a light house (not at all familiar) w ere employed. For t he scores on complexit y, a t riangle (not complex) and a comput er (complex) w ere used as examples. In order t o reduce response bias, t he subject s w ere encouraged not t o rat e all pict ures using t he same point s in t he familiarit y and complexit y scale, but t o use t he w hole range of re-sponses possible. The children gave t heir rere-sponses aloud and t he experim ent er ent ered t he inf orm at ion int o re-sponse sheet s.

b. Adult s: subject s w ere run in groups (16 t o 45 indi-viduals) in 5 sessions separat ed by 5 minut e int ervals. The 400 pict ures w ere project ed sequent ially for 10 seconds each on a w hit e surface using a slide project or. Subject s w ere inst ruct ed t o use answ er sheet s t hat w ere given t hem t o ent er t he name of t he object s t hat w ere depict ed on t he slides. They w ere specifically t old not t o w orry about spelling. If t hey w ere unable t o name t he draw ing, t hey w ere inst ruct ed t o put dow n “ don’t know t he name“ (DKN) or “ don’t know t he object ” (DKO). Familiarit y w it h t he concept and visual complexit y of pict ures w ere also scored using 5 point scales (1 represent ed t he least familiar and complex) and subject s w ere inst ruct ed t o t ry t o use t he full range of scoring point s. Adult part icipant s received t he same inst ruct ions and pract ice pict ures as t he chil-dren. We chose t o inst ruct children and adult s t o use a different number of point s for t he rat ings of familiarit y and visual complexit y [children (1, 3 and 5) and adult s (1 t o 5)] because Cycow icz et al.5 cit e t hat a pilot st udy show ed t hat children do not assign rat ings across t he full range of numerical values used by adult s.

Measures

The follow ing measures w ere obt ained for each pic-t ure:

a. M odal nam e: t he nam e given by t he m ajorit y of subject s. Spelling m ist akes w ere correct ed. When t he children’s modal names w ere different from t hat of t he adult s’ t hey w ere classified by 2 judges int o one of 5 cat -egories: synonym (including local subst it ut es such as t he Sout h American feline “ onça” for leopard), superordinat es (such as bug for ant ), subordinat e (apple t ree for t ree), coordinat es (same cat egory, such as cockroach for beet le), com ponent (part of nam es, such as ball f or f oot ball), or failures (including definit ions of object s, such as “ t o make t hings” , and similar object s, such as clock for compass). Discrepancies bet w een judges w ere resolved by a t hird judge.

b . Nam e agreem ent : ref ers t o t he d egree t o w hich sub ject s agree on t he nam e of t he p ict ure. Tw o m ea-sures w ere used: t he percent age of subject s w ho used

t he m odal name and the H index, calculated in the follow -ing manner:

This index t akes int o account t he num ber of subject s t hat gives each one of t he different nam es used for t he same pict ure3,5. k refers t o t he number of different names given t o each pict ure, and t he Pi is t he proport ion of sub-ject s w ho gave each name. DKN and DKO do not ent er int o t he com put at ion of t his index. The great er t he nam -ing agreement bet w een subject s, t he closer t he H is t o 0. c. Familiarit y: refers t o t he familiarit y of t he concept depict ed. Scores ranged from 1 t o 5 (1= very unfamiliar, 2= unfamiliar, 3= medium, 4= familiar, 5= very familiar).

d. Visual complexit y: refers t o t he amount of lines and det ails in t he draw ing. Scores ranged from 1 t o 5 (1= least complex; 5= most complex).

e. Lengt h of modal name: number of let t ers in t he m odal nam e. In som e cases, m ore t han one m odal nam e w as available, so t he m ean lengt h w as calculat ed.

Word frequency is not available in Port uguese so it w as not included in t his st udy. Naming lat ency w as not assessed because adult subject s w ere not evaluat ed indi-vidually. We chose not t o assess image consist ency be-cause it w ould m ake rat ing sessions far t oo long for bot h children and adult s. Also, because age of acquisit ion w as not assessed in children, w e opt ed t o exclude t his mea-sure f rom t he evaluat ion of adult s.

Statistical analysis

Involved pict ures as unit s of m easure. The hypot hesis of normalit y and equalit y of variance of scores of adult and children on t he 7 measures invest igat ed (percent age of subject s w ho used t he m odal nam e, t he H st at ist ics, familiarit y, visual complexit y, w ord lengt h, DKN and DKO) f o r t h e w h o l e 4 0 0 p i ct u r e-set w er e t est ed u si n g Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Levene t est s, respect ively. M ost measures did not show normal dist ribut ion or homocedas-t icihomocedas-t y so non paramehomocedas-t ric homocedas-t eshomocedas-t s w ere employed. Spearman rho correlat ions bet w een t he seven measures w ere car-ried out for dat a of adult s and children separat ely. Spear-man r correlat ions of scores of each variable bet w een chil-dren and adult s w ere also obt ained. Scores of chilchil-dren and adult s w ere compared using M ann-Whit ney U t est . The level of significance used w as 0.01 because of t he large number of comparisons conduct ed. Paramet ric t est s (Pearson correlat ions and St udent t t est s) w ere also used in order t o enable comparisons w it h previous report s on t he pict ure-set s [Pearson correlat ions and t w o-t ailed St u-dent t t est s for indepenu-dent samples w ere also conduc-t ed conduc-t o analyse all resulconduc-t s obconduc-t ained here. The paconduc-t conduc-t ern of effect s w as unchanged (dat a not show n except for corre-lat ions bet w een dat a of adult s and children; see Table 4].

RESULTS

Table 1 cont ains t he f ollow ing indices f or t he w hole 400 pict ure-set for bot h age groups: H index

H=Σ Pi log (1/ )2 Pi

k

(4)

(nam e agreem ent ), percent age of subject s produc-ing t he modal name, familiarit y, visual complexit y, name lengt h, DKN and DKO.

Informat ion on each of t he 400 pict ures for bot h age groups [H index (name agreement ), percent age of subject s producing t he modal name, familiarit y, visual complexit y, name lengt h, DKN and DKO] and all alt ernat ive names given can be found at ht t p:// w w w .epm.br/psico/PSICO.HTM .

The correlat ion analysis conduct ed t o det ermine t he degree of relat ionship bet w een m easures f or adults and children can be found in Tables 2 and 3, respectively. Table 4 show s the correlations and com-parisons of t he 7 measures bet w een adult s and chil-dren.

The M ann-Whit ney U t est s bet w een measures f rom t he dif f erent age groups show ed t hat t he children’s percent age of name agreement w as low er t han adult s’, w hile t he H index and use of DKO (p< 0.001) and DKN (p< 0.01) w ere low er for adult s. The children also rat ed pict ures as less fam iliar and less complex (p< 0.001) and used short er names for pict ures (p< 0.01).

Tw o concept s had no modal name for t he adult s (t w o nam es w ere used for each concept w it h t he same frequency). The modal names given by t he adult s t o 24 of t he pict ures w ere not correct t rans-lat ions of t he int ended names in English (see ht t p://

w w w .epm.br/psico/PSICO.HTM ). These misnomers w ere separat ed int o 3 groups:

a. Ambiguous pict ures: 56 (chisel, misint erpret ed as a screw driver), 262 (basin, m isint erpret ed as a bat h), 298 (paddle, as a racket ), 299 (parachut e, as a balloon), 334 (callipers, considered t w eezers), and 340 (cymbals, a spool of t hread).

b. Int ended names in English t hat can be t rans-lat ed int o Port uguese but t hat w ere nevert heless named w it h common local subst it ut es: pict ures 136 (leopard), 159 (ost rich), 364 (lizard) and 394 (vul-ture) w ere misinterpreted as common animals of the Brazilian fauna (“ onça, ema, lagart ixa, urubu” , re-spect ively). Pict ure 261 (acorn) w as mist aken for a w alnut , pict ure 297 (net ) w as named w it h t he w ord “ rede” rat her t han “ puçá” (t he specific, albeit relat i-vely unknow n name for t his concept in Port uguese), draw ing 330 (blow fish) w as nam ed fish, and pic-t ure 386 (squash) w as mispic-t aken for a pic-t ypical Soupic-t h Am erican veget able (“ chuchu” ). Sim ilar nam es of clot hes w ere used for pict ures 125 (jacket ) and 224 (sw eat er). Pict ure 141 (lips) w as described as a mout h, w hile pict ure 149 (mouse) w as considered a rat . Also, pict ure 99 (French horn) w as mist aken for a t rombone and pict ure 243 (t rumpet ) for a cornet . Pict ure 283 (fishing reel) w as also considered a spool of t hread. Pict ure 345 (easel) w as mist aken for a blackboard. Finally, t he 2 pict ures t hat received no

Table 1. Summary statistics for adults and children for the 400 picture set.

Nam e Nam e Familiarit y Com plexit y DKN DKO Lengt h

agreement (H) agreement (%)

adult s children adult s children adult s children adult s children adult s children adult s children adult s children M ean 0.79 1.13 0.77 0.60 4.00 3.38 2.65 2.42 0.04 0.08 0.03 0.10 6.99 6.45 SD 0.73 0.79 0.25 0.30 0.65 0.77 0.76 0.50 0.09 0.11 0.08 0.15 3.15 2.81

N 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400 400

(5)

modal name could also be included in this category: picture 183 (racoon) w as named w ith the same fre-quency as a skunk and fox (“ gambá” and “ raposa” ) and picture 271 (closet) as “ closet” and “ armário” (cupboard).

c. Words t hat do not exist in Port uguese: pict ure 144 (mit t en, considered a glove), pict ure 373 (pret -zel, t hought of as a biscuit ).

Children provided 103 modal names that differed from those of adults and failed to determine a modal n am e f o r an ext r a 2 0 co n cep t s (see h t t p :// w w w .epm.br/psico/PSICO.HTM ). Synonyms, compo-nent s and superordinat e mist akes w ere observed 14 times each. Coordinate misnomers w ere the most fre-quent (28 concepts), w hile only one concepts w as clas-sif ied as a subordinat e. The judges disagreed only on 5 of t hese classif icat ions.

Table 2. Spearman’s rho correlations among the measures obtained from adults (see note of table 1; *p<0.01).

H % Familiarit y Com plexit y Lengt h DKN DKO

H -0.967* -0.474* 0.269* 0.160* 0.583* 0.513*

% 0.558* -0.312* -0.185* -0.656* -0.595*

Familiarit y -0.643* -0.123 -0.732* -0.565*

Com plexit y 0.159* 0.390* 0.242*

Lengt h 0.162* 0.113

DKN 0.684*

DKO

Table 3. Spearman’s rho correlations among the measures obtained from children (see note of table 1; *p<0.01).

H % Familiarit y Com plexit y Lengt h DKN DKO

H -0.836* -0.389* 0.278* 0.027 0.429* 0.526*

% 0.590* -0.407* -0.122 -0.681* -0.768*

Familiarit y -0.600* -0.201* -0.614* -0.650*

Com plexit y 0.195* 0.356* 0.369*

Lengt h 0.236* 0.143*

DKN 0.676*

DKO

Table 4. Comparison of scores of each measure between adults and children using parametric and non-parametric analysis (i.e. Pearson/ Spearman correlations and Student t tests/Mann-Whitney U test; *p<0.01; **p<0.001).

Pearson correlat ion Spearman rho correlat ion Student t Test M ann-Whit ney U t est

H 0.563* 0.627* -6.34* * -6.12* *

% 0.585* 0.670* 8.42* * -7.51* *

Familiarit y 0.835* 0.855* 48.17* * -11.250* *

Com plexit y 0.849* 0.852* 23.48* * -4.15* *

Lengt h 0.628* 0.628* 2.57* -2.68*

DKN 0.476* 0.685* -4.47* * -2.59*

(6)

DISCUSSION

The dist ribut ion of H values for t he adult s has a low mean and is posit ively skew ed, indicat ing t hat concept s have a high name agreement overall. The H of t he children w as larger, show ing t hat name agreement is not as high as adult s’. These result s are in accord w it h Cycow icz et al.’s5 dat a for Nort h

American speaking subject s of t he same age groups.

The fact t hat among t he 400 pict ures only 24 did not ref lect t he exact t ranslat ion of t he int ended names in English by adult s in t he present st udy indi-cates that the majority of the pictures from Cycow icz et al.5 represent concept s t hat are know n by

Brazil-ian universit y st udent s. Am ong t he dif f erences in nam ing, 16 ref lect ed use of com m on local subst i-t ui-t es, ali-t hough all bui-t 2 had H values higher i-t han the 75th percentile (Q3, see Table 1), suggesting that t hese concept s had lit t le naming consist ency. The except ions w ere pict ures 141 (lips) and 149 (mouse), w hich w ere consist ent ly nam ed as m out h (‘boca” ) and rat (“ rat o” ), respect ively. While mout h and lips are cert ainly adequat e descript ions of pict ure 141, t he w ord for mouse in Port uguese, “ camundongo” , possibly because it is so long, is of t en subst it ut ed for “ rat o” (rat ). Pict ures 144 (mit t en) and 373 (pret -zel) do not exist in Port uguese and should t herefore be avoided in st udies in t his language. Our dat a fur-t her suggesfur-t s fur-t hafur-t picfur-t ures 56 (chisel), 262 (basin), 298 (paddle), 299 (parachut e), 334 (callipers), and 340 (cym bals), should not be em ployed in any lan-guage as they proved ambiguous concepts and w ere named as different objects entirely. Alario & Ferrand6,

w ho conduct ed a st udy of t he 400 pict ures using French adult s, replaced 7 pict ures (no. 19, 95, 96, 283, 288, 327, 373) because t hey had been select ed in t he “ American context” . In the present study, only 2 of t hese pict ures (no. 96 and 327) had H scores below t he 75% percent ile, suggest ing t hat t hey might in fact be inadequat e for ot her cult ures.

Children named 103 concept s different ly from adult s and failed t o det ermine a modal name for 30 pict ures. Considering t he t ype of m isnom ers em -ployed by children w hen compared t o adult s, w e found t hat most w ere coordinat e mist akes, w hich appeared t w ice as oft en as synonyms, component s, and superordinat es, indicat ing t hat children t end t o error by confusing object in t he same semant ic cat -egory. Only 30 of the 103 differences in naming w ere failures, and in most cases t he names at t ribut ed t o the pictures resembled the object depicted (e.g. clock for compass). Overall, dat a on differences in nam-ing by adult s and children are in agreement w it h

resu lt s o b t ain ed f o r t h e sam e ag e g ro u p s b y Cycow icz et al.5, w ho found t hat Nort h American

children named 90 concept s different ly from adult s, t hat t hese differences w ere m ost coordinat e m is-t akes, and is-t hais-t naming failures did nois-t appear is-t o ref lect percept ual or f unct ional dif f erences am ong age groups, but rat her a lack of know ledge of part i-cular concept s by t he young children. For a more det ailed discussion on t he com parison of Brazilian and North American children see M iranda et al. (sub-mit t ed)14.

The correlat ion analysis conduct ed t o det ermine t he degree of relat ionship bet w een m easures f or adult s and children separat ely (Tables 2 and 3) sho-w ed t hat t he 2 measures of name agreement (H and %) w ere highly negat ively correlat ed alt hough cor-relations w ere much smaller w hen naming w as com-pared bet w een t he age groups. Naming has been show n not t o correlat e as highly as familiarit y and visual com plexit y8 w hen dat a obt ained in different

languages8 and ages12 are cont rast ed. This w as

con-firmed by our result s in t erms of t he comparison bet w een adult s and young children. The names at -t ribu-t ed -t o pic-t ures may also differ among subjec-t s w ho speak t he same nat ive language but w ho in-habit dist inct regions or count ries, are from differ-ent social and educat ional background, of differdiffer-ent ages, and possibly genders. Hence, pilot studies w ith t he specif ic populat ion t o be invest igat ed m ust al-w ays be conduct ed so as t o det ermine t he adequacy of t he norm s for each language.

The follow ing findings of correlat ions of relat ive m agnit ude suggest t hat concept s w hich w ere dif f i-cult t o name are lit t le know n by t he t w o age groups: t he percent age of name agreement and familiarit y w ere negat ively correlat ed w it h DKN and DKO, t he H index w as posit ively correlat ed t o DKN and DKO, and percent age of name agreement w as correlat ed posit ively w it h familiarit y. Familiarit y w as also nega-tively correlated w ith visual complexity for adults and children, suggest ing t hat subject s find less complex f igures m ore f am iliar as discussed by Snodgrass & Vanderw art3 and Berman et al.12. The reason for t his

effect is unknow n but it may be relat ed t o differ-ences in t he charact erist ics of t he draw ings, such as t he number of det ails used t o depict t he object s12.

(7)

in cognit ive st udies in w hich name agreement must be high, even if t heir H index seem s adequat e.

The remaining correlat ions w ere small (< 0.4) but w it h few except ions w ere highly significant . Word lengt h seems t o represent an independent at t ribut e of the pictures from the other measures investigated in t he present st udy because it correlat ed lit t le or not at all w it h t hem.

The correlat ions bet w een measures of adult s and children analysed separat ely w ere generally higher t han t hose obt ained from adult s of different coun-t ries using smaller secoun-t s3,5-8,12,, and also t han t hose

observed in st udies using all 400 pict ures3,6. The

rea-son for t his may be t hat w e combined rat ings on all 400 pict ures in t he sam e analysis [unlike Cycow icz et al.5, w ho divided t hem int o 3 set s] and used t he

same subject s t o rat e naming, familiarit y and visual complexit y [unlike Alario & Ferrand6], t hus low ering

variance w hich increases correlat ions. Nevert heless, t he consist ency in t he pat t ern of correlat ions among measures report ed for different cult ures and age groups is st ill m aint ained here, support ing t he use-fulness of t he 400 pict ure-set as a t ool for cognit ive research.

When dat a of Brazilian children w ere compared t o t hat of adult s, t he correlat ions w ere smaller t han t hose observed by Berman et al.12, w ho cont rast ed

rat ings of Nort h American 7 t o 10 year-olds and adult s for t he combined analysis of set s of 259 and 61 pict ures. This difference in effect s may be due t o 4 fact ors, independent ly or in combinat ion: a) older children may use rat ings t hat are more similar t o t hose of adult s t han 5 t o 7 year-olds’; b) Berman et al.12 seemed t o have used paramet ric analysis t hat

t end t o lead t o higher correlat ions bet w een mea-sures; c) t he t hird pict ure-set of 79 pict ures, added t o t he previous t w o set s t o make up t he 400 pic-t ures used here, include conceppic-t s pic-t hapic-t are less fa-m iliar t o young children, fa-m ore dif f icult t o ident if y and have low er frequency counts5, and therefore may

decrease t he int ercorrelat ion bet w een t he measures obt ained from young children and adult s for t he w hole 400 pict ure-set ; d) t he fact t hat t he Univer-sit y st udent s in t he present st udy consist ed basically of females. It is not possible t o det ermine in w hat w ay t he use of f ew m en could bias t he result s re-port ed here for lit t le is know n about gender differ-ences in naming, especially w hen speeded responses are not required. In fact , t he st udies on Snodgrass and Vanderw art ’s and Cycow icz’s pict ures did not

t ake gender int o account w hen analysing t he dat a (see 3,5-8) and Sanfeliu & Fernandez8 and Alario &

Ferrand6 even failed t o report t he sex of t heir

volun-t eers. Unforvolun-t unavolun-t ely volun-t he classes of Psychology svolun-t u-dent s t est ed (t he chosen course in t he ot her publi-cations of adult norms for the picture sets) had more female subjects. With the children, w ho w ere assessed individually, it w as possible t o select volunt eers so as t o include t he sam e num ber of boys and girls.

The comparison betw een age groups using M ann-Whit ney U t est s show ed differences among all mea-sures albeit t he signif icant correlat ions bet w een children’s and adult s’ dat a. Children’s name agree-ment w as low er and t he use of DKN and DKO w as higher t han t hat of adult s, show ing t hat children produced more alt ernat ive names and had more dif-f icult y nam ing concept s. In addit ion, t he children rat ed pict ures as less familiar and less complex t han adult s, possibly due t o t heir t endency t o show a smaller range and less variat ion in t heir rat ings5. The

children also chose short er nam es f or pict ures, re-flect ing t hat t he vocabulary of 5 t o 7 year-olds con-sist s of short er w ords, possibly because t hey are ac-quired earliest and are bet t er represent ed in t heir lexicon5. Taken together, these results are in line w ith

data presented by Cycow icz et al.5, although in many

cases t heir dat a show ed only a t rend of difference bet w een young children and adult s w hile ours reached highly significant effect s. This is not surpris-ing since w e used all 400 pict ures in t he same analy-sis w hile Cycow icz et al.5 divided t hem int o 3 set s,

w hich could have enhanced variances as discussed above. This illust rat es t he import ance of using larger amount s of pict ures w hen different populat ions are t o be compared.

We hope t hat t he normat ive dat a present ed here w ill be used as a guide f or t he select ion of pict orial st im uli t o be used in research in several f ields11.

In-vest igat ors w ho int end t o st udy semant ic memory using pict ured object s can f ind pict ures organised int o sem ant ic cat egories in Cycow icz et al.’s5 paper.

Those int erest ed in repet it ion prim ing should con-sult Snodgrass & Corw in15 for t he 150 pict ures t hat

present low t o moderat e complexit y and sufficient area to enable the creation of distinctly different frag-ment ed images.

(8)

REFERENCES

1. Johnson CJ, Clark JM, Paivio A. Cognitive components of picture nam-ing. Psychol Bull 1996;120:113-139.

2. Murtha S, Chertkow H, Beauregard M, Evens A. The neural substrate of picture naming. J Cogn Neurosci 1999;11:399-423.

3. Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M. A standardised set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual com-plexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 1980;6:174-215.

4. Theios J, Amrhein PC. Theoretical analysis of the cognitive processing of lexical and pictorial stimuli: reading, naming, and visual and con-ceptual comparisons. Psychol Rev 1989;96:5-24.

5. Cycowicz, Y. M., Friedman, D., Rothstein, M., & Snodgrass, J.G. Pic-ture naming by young children: norms for name agreement, familiar-ity, and visual complexity. J Exp Child Psychol 1997;65:171-237. 6. Alario F-X, Ferrand L. A set of 400 pictures standardised for French:

norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual com-plexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behav Res Method Instrum Comput 1999;31:351-552.

7. Barry C, Morrison CM, Ellis AW. Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: effects of age of acquisition, frequency, and name agreement. Quart J Exp Psychol 1997;50A:560-585.

8. Sanfeliu MC, Fernandez A. A set of 254 Snodgrass-Vanderwart pic-tures standardised for Spanish. Behav Res Method Instrum Comput 1996;28:537-555.

9. Matsukawa J. A study of characteristics of pictorial material. Memoirs of the faculty of Law and Literature: Shimane University. Shimane-ken, Japan, 1983.

10. Pind J, Jonsdottir H, Tryggvadottir HB, Jonsson F. Icelandic norms for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) pictures: name and image agreement, familiarity, and age of acquisition. Scand J Psychol 2000;41:41-48. 11. Pompéia S, Bueno OFA. Preliminary adaptation into Portuguese of a

standardised picture set for use in research and neuropsychological assessment. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1998;56:366-374.

12. Berman S, Friedman D, Hamberger M, Snodgrass JG. Developmental picture norms: relationship between name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity for child and adult ratings of two sets of line draw-ings. Behav Res Method Instrum Comput 1989;21:371-382.

13. Brito GNO. The Conner’s abbreviated teacher rating scale: develop-ment of norms in Brazil. J Abn Child Psychol 1987;15:511-518. 14. Miranda MC, Pompéia S, Bueno OFA. Norms for a 400 picture set in

Imagem

Table 1. Summary statistics for adults and children for the 400 picture set.
Table 3. Spearman’s rho correlations among the measures obtained from children (see note of table 1; *p&lt;0.01).

Referências

Documentos relacionados

9.. Entende que a qualidade do atendimento oferecida pela Postal Saúde baixou e reivindica a volta dos Correios Saúde. Quanto a segunda proposta, solicitou que os Correios

Quando o organismo recebe mais hidratos de carbono, do que os que pode utilizar para produção de energia, ou depositar sob a forma de glicogénio, o excesso é convertido

É dessa articulação entre professores e alunos que, como “fruto de um diálogo secular” (CHERVEL, 1990, p.222), vão se constituindo as disciplinas escolares e

Sócios da Sociedade Limitada de nome empresarial RIBEIRO E ANJOS EMPREENDIMENTOS E ENGENHARIA LTDA, registrada legalmente por contrato social devidamente arquivado nesta

A infestação da praga foi medida mediante a contagem de castanhas com orificio de saída do adulto, aberto pela larva no final do seu desenvolvimento, na parte distal da castanha,

b) Organizações do setor voluntário – Aplicação de formulário com questões abertas e fechadas, conforme mostra o Apêndice A, a 28 pessoas sendo: uma

O evento será organizado dentro do escopo das prescrições do Código Desportivo do Automobilismo 2015, dos Regulamentos Desportivo e Técnico do Campeonato Brasileiro

A fim de avaliar o aproveitamento do amendoim, foi estudado o potencial do extrato aquoso do amendoim como ingrediente funcional em leites fermentados, contribuindo assim,