• Nenhum resultado encontrado

J. Pediatr. (Rio J.) vol.91 número4

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2018

Share "J. Pediatr. (Rio J.) vol.91 número4"

Copied!
7
0
0

Texto

(1)

www.jped.com.br

ORIGINAL

ARTICLE

Parental

tobacco

consumption

and

child

development

Nadine

F.

Santos

a,∗

,

Raquel

A.

Costa

a,b

aUniversidadedeTrás-os-MonteseAltoDouro,VilaReal,Portugal

bUniversidadeEuropeiaLaureateInternationalUniversities,Lisbon,Portugal

Received3June2014;accepted17September2014 Availableonline27February2015

KEYWORDS

Psychomotor development;

Tobaccouse;

Parenting

Abstract

Objective: Toanalyzetheassociationbetweenparentaltobaccoconsumptionandthe preva-lenceofpsychomotordevelopmentdisordersinchildrenbetween6and22monthsofage.

Method: Onehundredandninemothers,fathers,andtheirbabiesparticipatedinthestudy.The sociodemographicandclinicalconditionswereassessedusingquestionnaires.Tobacco consump-tionwasassessedusingtheFagerströmTestforNicotineDependence(FTND).Childdevelopment wasevaluatedusingtheScaleofPsychomotorDevelopmentinEarlyChildhood.

Results: There was a significant negative correlation between the father’s morning smok-ing(FTND)andthechild’slanguagedevelopmentquotient;r=-0.41,p=0.005,r2=0.15.The

childrenofmotherswithoutnicotinedependencehadahighermeanlanguagedevelopment quo-tientthanchildrenofmotherswithnicotinedependence;F(1,107)=5.51,p=0.021,␩p2=0.05. Conclusion: Parentalsmokingappearstohaveadetrimentaleffectonchilddevelopment. ©2015SociedadeBrasileiradePediatria.PublishedbyElsevierEditoraLtda.Allrightsreserved.

PALAVRAS-CHAVE

Desenvolvimento psicomotor;

Consumodetabaco;

Parentalidade

Consumodetabacoparentaledesenvolvimentoinfantil

Resumo

Objetivo: Analisararelac¸ãoentreoconsumodetabacoparentaleaprevalênciadedistúrbios nodesenvolvimentopsicomotoremcrianc¸asentreosseiseosvinteedoismesesdeidade.

Método: Cento e nove mães, pais e seus bebês participaram no estudo. Ascircunstâncias sociodemográficas e clínicas foram avaliadas com recurso a questionários. O consumo de tabacofoiavaliadoutilizandooTestedeFagerströmparaaDependênciaTabágica(Heatherton, Kozlowski,Frecker,&Fagerström,1991).Odesenvolvimentoinfantilfoiavaliadoutilizandoa EscaladoDesenvolvimentoPsicomotordaPrimeiraInfância(Brunet&Lézine,1951).

Pleasecitethisarticleas:SantosNF,CostaRA.Parentaltobaccoconsumptionandchilddevelopment.JPediatr(RioJ).2015;91:366---72. ∗Correspondingauthor.

E-mail:nadifernandessantos@gmail.com(N.F.Santos).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jped.2014.09.006

(2)

Resultados: Háumacorrelac¸ão negativasignificativaentreofumo matinal(FTND) dopaie o quociente de desenvolvimentode linguagem da crianc¸a, r=-0,41, p=0,005, r2=0,15. As

crianc¸asdemãessemdependênciatabágicatêmemmédiaumquocientededesenvolvimento delinguagemsuperioràscrianc¸asdemãescomdependênciatabágica,F(1,107)=5,51,p=0,021,

␩p2=0,05.

Conclusão: Oconsumodetabacoparentalpareceterumefeitoprejudicialparao desenvolvi-mentodacrianc¸a.

©2015SociedadeBrasileiradePediatria.PublicadoporElsevierEditoraLtda.Todososdireitos reservados.

Introduction

Development inthe firstyearsof lifeis essential. Several

environmental factors, such as the parental consumption

ofsubstances,canincrease thelikelihoodof

developmen-tal difficulties in childhood,1 especially in the emotional,

educational,2social,behavioral,andpsychologicallevels.3

Tobacco consumption, specifically---which has high preva-lence in Portugal (22%)4---is an important public health

problem, and has been associated with difficulties in self-regulation; increased excitability and activation in the neonatal period;5 lower birth weight;6,7 learning

difficulties;6 lower volume of the frontal and cerebellar

lobes---responsiblefor emotional functioning,impulse con-trol,andattention;8smallerheadcircumference;9cognitive

andlanguageneurodevelopmentaldisorders;10,11and

child-hoodemotionalandbehavioraldisorders.6Thisassociation

canbeexplainedbythefactthatduringchildhood,thebrain continuesto develop andis particularlysensitive to envi-ronmentalpollutants,6orbybraindisordersresultingfrom

exposuretonicotineduringpregnancy.8

Asenvironmentalriskfactorsappeartoberelatedto chil-dren’sdevelopmentaldisorders,especiallymotor,language, social, cognitive, behavioral, and psychological disorders, thestudyofpsychomotordevelopmentassociatedtothese factorsis the aim of thisstudy.Although thereis a great dealofliteraturerelatedtochilddevelopment,the associ-ationbetweentobaccousebybothparentsandpsychomotor developmentisstilllargelyunknown.Therefore,thisstudy differs from previous studies due to the fact that (1) it analyzes several aspects of child development - posture, language,visual-motorcoordination,andsocial---ratherthan beinglimitedtooveralldevelopment;(2)moststudiesfocus on the effects of this consumption on children’s health, and/orduringpregnancyandnotafterit;(3)bothparents wereconsidered.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from four day care centers

located in the city of Funchal-Madeira, Portugal, after

authorizationbytheDirec¸ãoRegionaldeEducac¸ão(Regional

Education Board). Most of the participants were of

Por-tugueseorigin(94.3%)andwhite(98.0%).

Participationinthestudy wasproposedto124 mothers

and124fathers;87.9%agreedtoparticipate,9.3%refused

toparticipateduetolackoffreetime,and2.8%werenot

interestedin participating. Thus,the sample consisted of

109mothers,109fathers,and109babies.Thesample

inclu-sioncriterionwas: (1) tobethemother/father ofa child

agedbetween6and22months;theexclusioncriteriawere:

(1)illiteracyand(2)theexistenceofdiseasesinthebabies.

Thestudywasperformedduring2011andthedatacollection

phaselastedthreemonths.

Tools

Sociodemographicandclinicaldata

Aquestionnairewasusedtocollectsocialanddemographic

data(age,gender,maritalstatus,yearsofeducation,

profes-sionalstatus,physical andpsychologicaldiseases, medical

or psychological treatment, numberof pregnancies,

num-berof miscarriages, number of children, age of children,

andchildren’sphysicalandpsychologicaldiseases),aswell

as clinical information on the pregnancy and newborn

(pregnancy planning, prenatal care, risk pregnancy,

ges-tational age, type of delivery, type of anesthesia, Apgar

score,weightandheight,headcircumference,reanimation,

healthproblemsatbirth,currentsleeppattern---monophasic

(longperiodsofcontinuoussleep)vs.biphasic(alternation

between periods of sleep and wakefulness), and type of

feeding.

Nicotinedependence

Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND)12,13

Thistestwasdevelopedtocompensateforthe

psychomet-riclimitationsoftheFagerströmToleranceQuestionnaire,12

and aims to measure the nicotine dependence of an individual.13 It consists of six items related to smoking

habits and behaviors, rated on a Likert scale ranging from0-3points.Higher resultsindicateagreater smoking dependence,14 in which a score of 0-3 indicates absence

of nicotine dependence, 4-6 indicates moderate nicotine dependence,and≥7indicatesseverenicotinedependence. ThePortugueseversionhasacceptablepsychometric prop-erties,withCronbach’s␣=0.66.13Test-retestreliabilitywas

ensuredbycorrelationvaluesoftheoriginalscaleof0.99. Factor analysis showed the existence of two factors: (1) cigaretteconsumption---dailyconsumptionpatterns---and(2) morningsmoking- degree of urgencyto restorethe level ofnicotineafterthenighttimeabstinence.13Inthepresent

(3)

Psychomotordevelopment

ScaleofPsychomotorDevelopmentinEarlyChildhood15

This scale allows the evaluation of the child’s

develop-mentallevel(1-30months)ineachofthefollowingareas:

postural---child’s movements such as rolling over, sitting,

and walking; visual-motor coordination---manipulation of

objects,visual-manualcoordination,andsolvingproblems;

language---expressionand understanding; and social---social

andpersonalrelations,especiallyadaptationtosocial

situa-tions,games,andrelationswithothers.15

Itconsistsof150 items,whicharedistributedin levels (1-30).Thisscaleallowsthecalculationofthe Developmen-talQuotient(DQ=DA×100/CA),inwhichDArepresentsthe developmentalageandCAthechronologicalage.ADQ≤75 indicateslowerdevelopment,whereasDQof76to100 indi-catesnormaldevelopment,and>100demonstrateshigher development.15

The scale validity criterion is 0.68, with a test-retest correlationcoefficientof0.85.15

Procedures

Allevaluationprocedureswereperformedandapprovedby

theethicscommitteeoftheinstitution.Mothersandfathers

received an explanation on the purpose and procedures

of thestudy, andafter the informedconsent wassigned,

theywereaskedtocompleteasocio-demographic/clinical

questionnaire and the FTND12,13 (approximate duration of

20-25minutes).

The Scale of Psychomotor Development in Early Childhood15 was used to assess the infant’s psychomotor

development. This assessment was always performed by thesame investigator, appropriately trained and familiar-izedwiththescale applicationprocedures.Eachbabywas observedindividually and only ata specific time (approx-imately 30to 40minutes), while thedaycare professional waspresent, but away fromthe baby’s visual range. The questionspertainingtoeachsubtestwereansweredbythe daycareprofessionalattheendofeachobservation.

Statistical

procedures

Pearson’sCorrelationtestwasusedtoassesswhetherthere

wasacorrelationbetweenthedegreeofnicotine

depend-ence(FTND)of themotherandthe fatherandthe child’s

postural,visual-motor,language,social,andoverall

devel-opmentquotient.16

The significance of nicotine dependence factor (FTND ≥ 7) of the mother and father on the composite of the variableschild’spostural,visual-motor,language,andsocial developmentquotientwasevaluatedbyseveralMultivariate AnalysisofVariance(MANOVAs).17Thesignificanceofthe

dif-ferencebetweenthechild’soveralldevelopmentquotient accordingtonicotinedependenceofthemotherandfather (FTND≥7)wasevaluatedbyusingthet-testforindependent samples.16

The chi-squaredtest wasusedtoassesswhether there wasan association betweenchild’s overall developmental quotient(lower, normal,upper)and thenicotine depend-ence(FTND≥7)ofthemotherandfather.16

Effectsizesofeachoftheanalysesperformedand inter-preted based on the classification proposed by Maroco,17

KinnearandGray,18andCohen.19

Results

Mostpregnancieswereplanned(65.1%)anddesired(97.2%).

Mostmothershadprenatalcare(98.2%)andanormal

preg-nancy(79.8%),lasting37weeksormore(94.7%,M=39.34,

SD=1.70). Most babies were born by vaginal delivery

(62.3%), and 37.7% were born by cesarean section, with

mostmothersreceivingepiduralanesthesia(73.4%).Apgar

score valuesranged from 3-10 (M=8.96, SD=1.06) at the

firstminuteoflife(97.2%≥7)andbetween7-10(M=9.74,

SD=0.57) at the fifth minute. At birth, weight varied

between 1.990kg and 4.530kg (M=3.31, SD=0.45), and

mosthadabirthweight≥2.500kg(99.1%),whereasheight

ranged from 36.00-59.05cm (M=48.85, SD=3.06), and

head circumference ranged between 30-37cm (M=34.64,

SD=1.22).Mostbabiesdidnotrequirereanimationatbirth

(97.2%) and had no health problems (95.4%). Most had a

pattern ofbiphasic sleep(59.6%).Mostmothersbreastfed

(81.7%)foraperiodoftimerangingfromonemonthto22

months(M=7.29,SD=5.29).

Maternalagerangedbetween19and45years(M=33.17,

SD=5.88)andpaternalagerangedbetween20and50years

(M=36.29,SD=6.03).Thesociodemographiccharacteristics

areshowninTable1.

Babieswereagedbetween6and22months,withmost being aged ≥ 12 months (M=14.50, SD=4.62). Approxi-matelyhalfweremales(50.5%;Table1).

AssociationbetweenMaternalandPaternal NicotineDependence(FTND)andChild DevelopmentQuotient

There was no statistically significant correlation between

the mother’s nicotine dependence and cigarette smoking

(FTND)andthepostural,visual-motor,language,social,and

overall childdevelopmentquotient,withsmalleffectsize

(r2 0.1). There was no statistically significant

correla-tion between the mother’s morning smoking (FTND) and

the postural, visual-motor, language, social, and overall

child development quotient, with small effect size (r2

0.1).However,therewasanegativeandmarginally

signif-icant correlation between the mother’s morning smoking

(FTND)andthechild’slanguagedevelopmentquotient,with

medium-sizedeffectsize(r2=0.11)(Table2).Thus,ahigher

degreeofmaternalmorningsmokingwasassociatedwitha lowerlanguagedevelopmentquotient.

(4)

Table1 Sociodemographicdata. Mother (n=109)

Father (n=109)

% %

Age

<36 58.7 45.0

≥36 41.3 55.0

Yearsofschooling

<9 20.2 31.2

≥9 79.8 68.8

Maritalstatus

Married 82.6 82.6

Single 11.9 11.9

Separated/Divorced 5.5 5.5

Professionalstatus

Student 1.8

-Employed 74.3 82.6

Unemployed 19.3 17.4

Homemaker 4.6

-Physicaldiseases

Yes 1.8 7.3

No 98.2 92.7

Psychologicaldiseases

Yes 0.9

-No 99.1 100

Medical/psychologicaltreatment

Yes 7.3 5.5

No 92.7 94.5

Numberofchildren

1 40.4 40.4

2 35.8 38.5

3ormore 23.8 21.1

M(SD) Numberofpreviouspregnancies 2.23(1.27) Numberofpreviousmiscarriages 0.27(0.63)

smoking were associated with a lower language

develop-mentquotient.

ImpactofMaternalandPaternalNicotine

Dependence(FTND)ontheChild’sDevelopment Quotient

Thematernalnicotinedependencefactor(FTND<7vs.FTNP

≥7)hadastatisticallysignificant effectonthe

multivari-atecompositeofchilddevelopmentquotient,␭=0.91,F(4,

104)=2.59, p=0.041, ␩p2=0.09, with the eta value

sug-gestingamedium-sizedeffect.Univariateanalysisshowed

thatchildrenofmotherswithoutnicotinedependencehad

ahighermeanvisual-motor development,F(1,107)=4.61,

p=0.034andlanguagequotientF(1,107)=5.51, p=0.021,

than children of mothers withnicotine dependence, with

theetavalue suggestingsmalleffectsize(Table3).There

werenosignificantdifferencesbetweenchildrenof moth-erswithnicotinedependencevs.childrenofmotherswith no nicotine dependence in terms of overall development

quotient,t(107)=1.16,p=0.248;however,Cohen’sd-value suggestsamedium-sized effect(Table3).The children of motherswith nonicotine dependence had a highermean overalldevelopmentquotientthanchildrenofmotherswith nicotinedependence.

Paternalnicotinedependencefactor (FTND<7vs.FTNP ≥ 7) did not have a statistically significant effect on themultivariatecompositeofchilddevelopmentquotient, ␭=0.99, F(4, 104)=0.32, p=0.865, ␩p2=0.01, with the eta value suggesting a small effect size. There were no significant differences between children of fathers with nicotinedependence vs.children of fatherswithno nico-tinedependenceintermsofoveralldevelopmentquotient, t(107)=0.16,p=0.875,withCohen’sd-valuesuggestinga smalleffectsize(Table3).

AssociationbetweenMaternalandPaternal NicotineDependence(FTND)andChild DevelopmentQuotient

There was no significant association between the overall

developmentquotientofthechildandthematernalnicotine

dependence(FTND<7vs.FTNP≥7),␹2(2)=1.56,p=0.459,

andthepaternaldependence, ␹2(2)=1.01,p=0.605, with

smalleffectsize(Table4).

Discussion

This study was conducted to analyze the association

between parental consumption of tobacco and child

development. The results show that there was such an

association,withparentaldailyconsumptionpattern,

nico-tinedependence,andtheurgency ofconsumption aftera

nocturnalabstinenceperioddemonstratingassociationwith

language difficulties; there was an especially significant

associationbetweenmaternalnicotinedependenceand

lan-guage,visual-motor,andglobaldevelopment.Theseresults

areespeciallyimportantbecausetheyshowthattheeffect

of parentalnicotine dependence is harmful not only

dur-ingpregnancy,asotherstudieshaddemonstrated10butalso

afterdelivery.In fact, thisstudy does notclarify the dif-ferentialeffectof prenatalor postnatalmaternalnicotine dependenceonchilddevelopment.However,when consid-eringthe effectofpaternalsmokingdependence onchild development,itcanbeassumedthatthisreferstothe post-natal period, which draws attention to the vulnerability of the newborn topassive exposure to parental smoking. The mechanisms that explain the effect of tobacco con-sumption during pregnancy on the fetus have been well studiedin recent years, andthereis some evidence indi-catingthattheconsumption ofnicotineandother tobacco components influence gestational duration,20 have toxic

effectsonfetalbraindevelopmentduringpregnancy,10and

areassociatedwithfetalhypoxia,changesintheserotonin uptake,changesinthedopaminergicsystems,andchanges inDNAandRNA synthesisin thebrain.17 Nicotineappears

totarget specific neurotransmitter receptors in the fetal brain, causing abnormalities in cell proliferation and dif-ferentiation,resultingincellnumberdeficitsandchanges in synapticactivity,21 impairingthe fetal-placental

(5)

Table2 Associationbetweennicotinedependence,cigaretteconsumption,morningtobaccoconsumption,andchild develop-mentquotient.

Developmentquotient Nicotinedependence Cigaretteconsumption Morningsmoking

r p r2 r p r2 r p r2

MOTHER(n=109)

Postural -0.17 0.399 -0.01 -0.13 0.514 -0.02 -0.26 0.196 0.03 Visual-Motor -0.09 0.650 -0.03 -0.06 0.785 -0.04 -0.18 0.382 -0.01 Language -0.29 0.143 0.05 -0.25 0.215 0.02 -0.37 0.055 0.11 Social -0.10 0.621 -0.03 -0.05 0.792 -0.04 -0.23 0.241 0.02 Overall -0.14 0.486 -0.02 -0.09 0.661 -0.03 -0.27 0.176 0.04

FATHER(n=109)

Postural 0.02 0.896 -0.02 0.01 0.943 -0.02 -0.02 0.899 -0.02 Visual-Motor 0.03 0.841 -0.02 0.01 0.955 -0.02 0.04 0.808 -0.02 Language -0.31 0.035 0.08 -0.30 0.043 0.07 -0.41 0.005 0.15 Social -0.22 0.144 0.03 -0.27 0.072 0.05 -0.16 0.298 0.00 Overall -0.13 0.389 -0.01 -0.15 0.305 0.00 -0.15 0.320 0.00

r2,sizeeffectcoefficientdeterminant. Pearson’scorrelationtest.

Table3 Parentalnicotinedependenceandchilddevelopmentquotient. Development

quotient

MOTHER FATHER

Without nicotine dependence (FTND<7) (n=82)M (SD)

With nicotine dependence (FTND≥7)

(n=27)M (SD)

p ␩p2 Without

nicotine dependence (FTND<7) (n=82)M (SD)

With nicotine dependence (FTND≥7)

(n=27)M (SD)

p ␩p2

Posturala 97.61(16.54) 98.04(17.49) 0.909 0.00 98.79(17.36) 96.24(15.81) 0.433 0.01

Visual-motora

91.29(12.20) 85.59(11.19) 0.034 0.04 90.71(12.71) 88.74(12.19) 0.405 0.01

Languagea 84.55(16.36) 76.07(15.96) 0.021 0.05 83.16(15.94) 81.48(17.60) 0.604 0.00

Sociala 92.20(12.96) 88.00(12.67) 0.145 0.02 92.33(13.42) 89.54(12.55) 0.269 0.01

d d

Overallb 91.50(14.27) 88.00(11.14) 0.248 0.27 90.81(15.24) 90.39(11.12) 0.875 0.03

FTND,FagerströmTestforNicotineDependence;M,mean;SDstandarddeviation;␩p2,etasquared---sizeeffectforMANOVA;d,Cohen’s d-value---sizeeffectfort-test.

aMANOVA.

b t-testforindependentsamples.

Table4 Associationbetweenparentalnicotinedependenceandchilddevelopmentquotient.

MOTHER FATHER

Development Quotient

Withoutnicotine dependence

Withnicotine dependence

Withoutnicotine dependence

Withnicotine dependence (FTND<7)(n=82) (FTND≥7)(n=27) (FTND<7)(n=63) (FTND≥7)(n=46)

% % V % % V

.12 .10

LowerDQa 7.3 7.4 7.9 6.5

NormalaDQa 70.7 81.5 69.9 78.3

UpperDQa 22.0 11.1 22.2 15.2

(6)

for the newborn.23 As a consequence, there are

signifi-cant changesin thebrain physiologyresponsiblefor basic perceptual skills;24 loss of centralnervoussystem cells in

the postnatal period; reduction of the frontal lobe and cerebellarvolumesresponsibleforattention,emotion,and impulsecontrol;8aswellasasmallerheadcircumferencein

newborns.9Thesephysiological alterationsmaybe

respon-siblefor developmentaldisordersfoundin thesechildren, suchasself-regulatorydifficulties, increasedexcitability,5

decrease in cognitive functions, and learning and mem-orydeficits.10,11Overalldevelopmentaldifficultiesobserved

inthis study,withemphasisonlanguageandvisual-motor skills, are consistent with previous studies that indicate an association between parental consumption of tobacco andchilddevelopmentdisorders,25especiallypsychomotor

disorders.26 Regarding the effect of tobacco consumption

bytheparents,theeffectiscausedbyexposureafterbirth, whichindicatesthenewborn’sspecificvulnerabilityto expo-suretotoxinsreleasedbytobaccoconsumption.27 Because

ofthe implications,parentalsmokingduringtheperinatal periodisanimportantpublichealthproblem,andtherefore interventionprogramsinperinatalhealththattarget smok-ingcessation,notonlyforfuturemothersbutalsoforfuture fathers,shouldbeconsideredaprioritytoensureabetter qualityoflifeforfamilies.

Despitethelimitationsofthisstudy---especiallybecause it did not consider a detailed analysis of the behavior associatedwithtobaccoconsumptionorregardingparental consumptionhistory---it shouldbeconsideredan important contributiontothestudyoftheeffectofparentalsmoking, asithelpstobetterunderstandtheeffectofconsumption notonlybymothers,butalsobyfathers,thus encompass-ingbothparentsinordertoanalyzetheindividualeffecton psychomotordevelopment.

Future studies should assess not only the differential effectofconsumption byeachparent,butalsothe differ-entialeffectofprenatalandpostnataltobaccoconsumption onchilddevelopment.

Conflicts

of

interest

Theauthorsdeclarenoconflictsofinterest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like tothank the Regional Director of

EducationforallowingthestudytobecarriedoutinRegião

AutónomadaMadeira(RAM)daycarecenters.Theauthors

are alsovery grateful to the headsof educational

estab-lishments, teachers, and kindergarten assistants, and the

parents who allowed the observation of the psychomotor

developmentoftheirbabiesandspentpartoftheirprecious

timetoanswerthequestionnaire.

Thisstudywasdevelopedaspartoftheproject‘‘Therole

ofgenotype-environmentinteractionontheresilienceand

vulnerabilitytodevelopmentalandmentalhealthproblems

inthefirst18monthsoflife’’(PTDC/PSI-PCL/119152/2010)

fundedbytheFundac¸ãoparaaCiênciaeTecnologia

(Foun-dationforScienceandTechnology).

References

1.WillrichA,AzevedoCC,FernandesJO.Desenvolvimentomotor na infância: influência dos fatores de risco e programas de intervenc¸ão.RevNeurocienc.2008;17:51---6.

2.BlanchardKA,SextonCC,MorgensternJ,McVeighKH,McCrady BS, Morgan TJ, et al. Children ofsubstance abusing women onfederalwelfare:implicationsforchildwell-beingandTANF policy.JHumBehavintheSocialEnv.2005;12:89---110. 3.WellsK.Substanceabuseandchildmaltreatment.PediatrClin

NorthAm.2009;56:345---62.

4.BridgeheadInternationalEuropequitting-progressand path-ways.London:Pfizer;2011March.ReportNo.USC1184. 5.StroudL,PasterR,PapandonatosG,NiauraR,SalisburyA,Battle

C,etal.Maternalsmokingduringpregnancyandnewborn neu-robehavior:effectsat10to27days.JPediatr.2009;154:10---6. 6.AgrawalA,ScherrerJ,GrantJ,SartorC,PergadiaM,Duncan A,etal.Theeffectsofmaternalsmokingduringpregnancyon offspringoutcomes.PrevMed.2010;50:13---8.

7.ChertokI,LuoJ,AndersonR.Associationbetweenchangesin smokinghabitsinsubsequentpregnancyandinfantbirthweight inWestVirginia.MaternChildHealthJ.2011;15:249---54. 8.Ekblad M, Korkeila J, Parkkola R, Lapinleimu H, Haataja

L, Lehtonen L, et al. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and regional brain volumes in preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2010;156:85---90.

9.KallenK.Maternalsmokingduringpregnancyandinfanthead circumferenceatbirth.EarlyHumDev.2000;58:197---204. 10.Kiechl-KohlendorferU,RalserE,PuppU,ReiterG,Griesmaier

E,TrawögerR.Smokinginpregnancy:ariskfactorforadverse neurodevelopmentaloutcomeinpreterminfants?ActaPaediatr. 2010;99:1016---9.

11.WehbyG,PraterK,McCarthyA,CastillaE,MurrayJ.Theimpact ofmaternalsmokingduringpregnancyonearlychild neurode-velopment.JHumCap.2011;5:207---54.

12.HeathertonTF,KozlowskiLT,FreckerRC,FagerströmKO.The Fagerström Test for NicotineDependence: a revisionof the FagerströmToleranceQuestionnaire.BritishJournalof Addic-tion.1991;86:1119---27.

13.FerreiraPL,QuintalC,LopesI,TaveiraN.Testede dependên-ciaànicotina:validac¸ãolinguísticaepsicométricadotestede Fagerström.DependênciaTabágica.2009;27:37---56.

14.FagerströmKO,KunzeM,SchoberbergerR,BreslauN,Hughes JR,HurtRD,etal.Nicotinedependenceversussmoking preva-lence:comparisonsamongcountriesandcategoriesofsmokers. TobaccoControl.1996;5:52---6.

15.BrunetO,LézineI.Ledevelopmentpsychologiquedela pre-mièreenfance.Paris:PressesUniversitairesdeFrance;1951. 16.MartinsC.Manualdeanálisededadosquantitativoscomrecurso

aoIBMSPSS:saberdecidir,fazer,interpretareredigir.Braga: Psiquilibrios;2011.

17.MarocoJ. Análiseestatísticacomutilizac¸ãodo SPSS.Lisboa: Edic¸õesSílabo;2007.

18.KinnearPR,GrayCD.PASWstatistics17madesimple.NewYork: PsychologyPress;2010.

19.CohenJ.Statisticalpoweranalysisforthebehavioralsciences. NewJersey:LawrenceErlbaumAssociates,Inc;1988. 20.Mello PR, Pinto GR, Botelho C. Influência do tabagismo na

fertilidade, gestac¸ão elactac¸ão. JPediatr (Rio J). 2001;77: 257---64.

21.Slotkin TA. Fetalnicotine or cocaineexposure:which one is worse?JPET.1998;285:931---45.

22.Jauniaux E,BiernauxV,Gerlo E,Gulbis B. Chronicmaternal smokingandcordbloodaminoacidandenzymelevelsatterm. ObstetGynecol.2001;97:57---61.

(7)

24.Key A, FergusonM, Molfese F,Peach K, Lehman C, Molfese V. Smoking during pregnancy affects speech-processing abil-ity in newborn infants. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115: 623---9.

25.Poole-DiSalvoE,LiuYH,BrennerS,WeitzmanM.Adult house-holdsmokingisassociatedwithincreasedchildemotionaland behavioralproblems.JDevBehavPediatr.2010;31:107---15.

26.Andraca I,Pino P, La ParraA, Rivera F,Castillo M.Factores de riesgo para el desarrollo psicomotor em lactentes naci-dos em óptimas condiciones biológicas. Rev Saude Publica. 1998;32:138---47.

Imagem

Table 1 Sociodemographic data. Mother (n = 109) Father(n= 109) % % Age &lt; 36 58.7 45.0 ≥ 36 41.3 55.0 Years of schooling &lt; 9 20.2 31.2 ≥ 9 79.8 68.8 Marital status Married 82.6 82.6 Single 11.9 11.9 Separated/Divorced 5.5 5.5 Professional status Stude
Table 2 Association between nicotine dependence, cigarette consumption, morning tobacco consumption, and child develop- develop-ment quotient.

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Table 2 The association between potential risk groups and daily smoking prevalence, nicotine dependence prevalence among daily smokers, and nicotine dependence prevalence in the

Validação do escore VI-RADS para estadiamento das neoplasias vesicais xx p.; 3 il.; 30 cm Dissertação de mestrado profissional apresentada à Faculdade de Medicina de Ribeirão Preto

In this article, we discuss the characteristics of smoking among women, addressing factors such as smoking prevalence, nicotine dependence, the role of the tobacco industry,

* Used to discriminate between smoking and non-smoking or between environments exposed versus unexposed to cigarette smoke. For smoking cars, the older the car and the greater

Results: There were no statistically significant correlation between gestational age and conceptional age and cerebral palsy; the electroencephalographic background activity

fazer alguma coisa hoje para não deixar a Lagoa morrer&#34;. Crescimento

1 An equally natural profit-sharing rule is based on the solution of the Nash bargaining game: firms get the profits that they would obtain under competition plus an equal share of