• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Sleep procrastination: an exploratory study with high school students

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2020

Share "Sleep procrastination: an exploratory study with high school students"

Copied!
31
0
0

Texto

(1)

Universidade do Minho

Escola de Psicologia

Vânia Cristina da Silva Ralha Mendes Cruz

Sleep procrastination: An exploratory study

with High School students

Vânia Cr uz Sleep procr as tination: An e xplor ator y study wit h High Sc hool s tudents

(2)
(3)

Vânia Cristina da Silva Ralha Mendes Cruz

Sleep procrastination: An exploratory study

with High School students

Dissertação de Mestrado

Mestrado Integrado em Psicologia

Trabalho efetuado sob a orientação do

Professor Doutor Pedro Rosário

e da

Doutora Paula Magalhães

Universidade do Minho

Escola de Psicologia

(4)

DIREITOS DE AUTOR E CONDIÇÕES DE UTILIZAÇÃO DO TRABALHO POR TERCEIROS Este é um trabalho académico que pode ser utilizado por terceiros desde que respeitadas as regras e boas práticas internacionalmente aceites, no que concerne aos direitos de autor e direitos conexos.

Assim, o presente trabalho pode ser utilizado nos termos previstos na licença abaixo indicada. Caso o utilizador necessite de permissão para poder fazer um uso do trabalho em condições não previstas no licenciamento indicado, deverá contactar o autor, através do RepositóriUM da Universidade do Minho.

Licença concedida aos utilizadores deste trabalho

Atribuição-NãoComercial-SemDerivações CC BY-NC-ND

(5)

Agradecimentos

“Estes anos são viagem…”. E que viagem maravilhosa que eu experienciei, que não teria sido possível sem o apoio incondicional da minha família. Aos meus pais, obrigada por todo o amor, carinho e suporte, mas essencialmente obrigada por sempre acreditarem em mim e me darem asas para voar, tendo sempre em vista o meu desenvolvimento e independência. Sem vocês não seria a pessoa que sou hoje. Obrigada aos meus irmãos, Bruno e Daniel, por serem os mais chatos e os melhores também.

Obrigada aos meus orientadores, Professor Doutor Pedro Rosário e Doutora Paula Magalhães, por toda a orientação durante este caminho. Estarei sempre grata pelo apoio continuo e pelas

aprendizagens que me transmitiram. Queria agradecer também ao GUIA, especialmente à Ana, à Clara e à Raquel, por terem partilhado comigo todas estas aprendizagens.

Ao André, por todo o carinho e suporte, mas especialmente por toda a paciência e companheirismo perante os momentos mais difíceis.

Por último, um obrigado a todos os meus amigos, de Barcelos e de Braga, por me terem apoiado durante esta jornada. Não teria sido a mesma experiência sem vocês ao meu lado para partilhar tristezas, sorrisos e aventuras.

(6)

STATEMENT OF INTEGRITY

I hereby declare having conducted this academic work with integrity. I confirm that I have not used plagiarism or any form of undue use of information or falsification of results along the process leading to its elaboration.

I further declare that I have fully acknowledged the Code of Ethical Conduct of the University of Minho.

University of Minho, October 18, 2019

Full name: Vânia Cristina da Silva Ralha Mendes Cruz

(7)

Procrastinação no sono: Um estudo exploratório com alunos do Ensino Secundário Resumo

A insuficiência do sono afeta a nossa saúde física e psicológica. Muitos fatores contribuem para a insuficiência do sono, mas um possível contribuinte pode ser de natureza motivacional, a procrastinação. A literatura sobre procrastinação do sono tem--se concentrado nos comportamentos que as pessoas praticam antes de ir para a cama (procrastinação na hora-de-dormir), excluindo comportamentos que as pessoas praticam quando estão na cama (procrastinação na cama). O principal objetivo deste estudo é explorar se esta procrastinação na cama é um fenómeno novo que se acrescenta ao processo de procrastinação na hora-de-dormir. O estudo foi realizado online com estudantes do ensino secundário incluindo um questionário de autorrelato com questões sociodemográficas, Escala de Procrastinação na Hora de Dormir, Escala de Procrastinação na Cama e variáveis contextuais (descritivas do sono, rotina e contexto familiar). A procrastinação na hora-de-dormir e a procrastinação na cama transmitem baixo nível de correlação. ANOVAs entre variáveis contextuais com os dois tipos de procrastinação no sono demonstraram que estas se relacionam de forma diferente. Estes resultados sugerem que avaliar a procrastinação do sono apenas como um comportamento de procrastinação antes de ir para a cama não é suficiente, adicionando uma nova perspetiva à literatura.

Palavras-chave: insuficiência de sono; procrastinação no deitar; procrastinação no sono; psicologia; saúde

(8)

Sleep procrastination: An exploratory study with High School students Abstract

Sleep insufficiency affects our physical and psychological health. Not only adults suffer the consequences of sleep deprivation, adolescents too are affected by this condition. Many factors contribute to sleep insufficiency, but one possible contributor may be of a motivational nature, namely procrastination. So far, sleep procrastination literature has focused on behaviors individuals engage in before going to bed (bedtime procrastination), but not on the behaviors individuals engage in after going to bed (while-in-bed procrastination). The main goal of this study is to explore whether this while-in-bed procrastination is a novel phenomenon that adds to the bedtime procrastination. The study was conducted online with high school students and comprised a self-report questionnaire with sociodemographic questions, Bedtime Procrastination Scale, While-in-bed Procrastination Scale, and contextual variables (related to sleep descriptives, routine related, and family related. Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination conveys a low correlation degree. Several ANOVAs were conducted to analyze differences in Bedtime and While-in-bed Procrastination; these analyses showed distinct results. Findings indicate that solely assessing Bedtime Procrastination as representing the procrastination of Sleep is limited. This study adds a new perspective to the literature by stressing the role of While-in-bed Procrastination, thus opening new research pathways.

Keywords: bedtime procrastination; health; sleep insufficiency; psychology; while-in-bed procrastination

(9)

Table of Contents

Agradecimentos _________________________________________________________ iii Abstract _______________________________________________________________ vi Sleep procrastination: An exploratory study with High School students __________________ 8 Purpose of the study _____________________________________________________ 11 Method ______________________________________________________________ 11 Results ______________________________________________________________ 14 Discussion ____________________________________________________________ 21 References ____________________________________________________________ 25

Index of Tables

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of sleep descriptives ________________________________ 14 Table 2 Descriptive statistics of family context ___________________________________ 15 Table 3 Descriptive statistics of routine. _______________________________________ 16 Table 4 Bedtime Procrastination Scale frequencies (Portuguese version) ________________ 18 Table 5 While-in-bed Procrastination Scale frequencies ____________________________ 19 Table 6 Bedtime Procrastination Scale and While-in-bed Procrastination Scale correlation ___ 20 Table 7. Analyses of variance_______________________________________________ 21

(10)

Running head: SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Sleep procrastination: An exploratory study with High School students

A considerable body of knowledge has highlighted the key role of sleep in maintaining our body homeostasis, as it contributes for a good performance of various functions, like metabolic functions, brain plasticity, energy conservation, and neuronal recuperation (Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Siegel, 2005; Siegel, 2009; Tononi & Cirelli, 2006). Sleep is also important for offline memory consolidation (Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Krueger et al., 2008; Maquet, 2001), cognitive abilities (Clemens, Fabó, & Halasz, 2005; Kirov, Uebel, Albrech, Banaschewski, & Rothenbergert, 2011; Schabus et al., 2006), consolidation of emotional memory (Nishida, Pearsall, Buckner, & Walker, 2009; Walker, 2009), and emotional processing (Gujar, McDonald, Nishida, & Walker, 2011; McNamara, McLaren, Smith, Brown, & Stickgold, 2005). Therefore, it comes as no surprise that sleep insufficiency has been recently coined as a major health issue and should be treated as a chronic disease.

Until recently, sleep insufficiency was regarded solely as a medical problem, with most research focused on individuals with sleep disorders (e.g., insomnia, apnea) (Kroese, De Ridder, Evers, & Adriaanse, 2014). However, sleep insufficiency affects not only people with sleep disorders but also the general population. For example, in the United States individuals are sleeping, on average, 6 hours or less per night, when the recommended amount of sleep is seven to nine hours for young adults and adults (Gallup, 2013; Krueger & Friedman, 2009; Sheehan, Frochen, Walsemann, & Ailshire, 2018; The National Sleep Foundation, 2015). This is a disturbing scenario as sleep insufficiency affects our social, physical, and psychological health and wellbeing, having already been appointed as a “major public health problem” (Centre for disease control and prevention, 2014). Sleep deprivation can have a toll on our concentration, carry memory and performance problems (Belenky et al., 2003; Ram, Seirawan, Kumar, & Clark, 2010;), contribute to anxiety, pain, depressive symptoms and suicidal ideation (Conklin, Yao, & Richardson, 2018; Roane & Taylor, 2008; Strin & Chapman, 2005), and lead to road accidents or mortality (Connor et. al, 2002). Furthermore, studies show that not sleeping enough plays an important role on our health; for example, can lead to hypertension (Gottlieb et al., 2006; Knutson et al., 2009), obesity (Nishiura & Hashimoto, 2010; Watanabe, Kikuchi, Tanaka, & Takahashi, 2010), diabetes (Holliday, Magee, Kritharides, Banks & Attia, 2013; Ohkuma et al., 2013), dyslipidemia (Petrov et al., 2013; Shin et al., 2016), and cardiovascular diseases (Buxton & Marcelli, 2010; Spiegel, Tasali, Penev, & van Cauter, 2004). Additionally, an unsuitable sleep hygiene compromises dietary intervention, thus contributing to weight gain (Nedeltcheva, Kilkus, Imperial, Schoeller, & Penev, 2010). Finally, sleep insufficiency is related to mortality triggered by drowsy driving. According to the National Sleep Foundation

(11)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Sleep in America poll (2005), 37% (or 103 million people) confirm to have fallen asleep while driving and 4% percent (11 million drivers) admit they have had an accident or near accident because they were too tired to drive. Disturbingly, between the years of 2011-2015, 3,662 out of 15,3297 accidents were due to drowsy driving, resulting in 4121 deaths (National Center for Statistics and Analysis, 2017).

Worryingly, not only adults suffer the consequences of sleep deprivation, adolescents too are affected by this condition. Adolescents are sleeping less than eigth hours per night, when the recommended amount of sleep for teenagers is eigth to 10 hours per night (Basch, Basch, Ruggles, & Rajan, 2014; Hirshkowitz et al., 2015). Literature shows that about 45% of adolescents between sixth and 12th grades are affected by sleep insufficiency (Gradisar, Gardner, & Dohnt, 2011; Ming et al., 2011).

Sleep insufficiency is being linked to risk-taking behavior; for example, high use of alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana, and high risky sexual behaviors (Moore & Meltzer, 2008; Shochat, Cohen-Zion, & Tzischinsky, 2014). Sleep insufficiency constitutes also a risk for obesity, depression, and suicide attempts (Chen, Burley, & Gotlib, 2012; Gupta, Mueller, Chan, & Meininger, 2002; Lofthouse, Gilchrist, & Splaingard, 2006; Owens & Adolescent Sleep Working Group , 2014).

Furthermore, academic performance of adolescents is being affected by this condition. Sleep insufficiency leads to emotional problems and injuries, sleepiness and lower motivation to study, all of which affect learning and academic performance (Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, 2018; Meijer, Habekothe, & Van Den Wittenboer, 2009; Roberts, Roberts, & Duong, 2009;). In fact, literature shows that adolescents’ poorer sleep quality, reduced sleep time, and excessive sleepiness is related to lower grades (Ming et al., 2011; National Sleep Foundation, 2006;). These consequences are expected to impact the adolescent’s development, growth, and functioning. (Brand & Kirov, 2011; Fallone, Owens, & Deane, 2002; Moore & Meltzer, 2008).

In sum, sleep insufficiency is a problem with major repercussions to individuals’ health, so it is important to further understand it. Ram, Seirawan, Kumar, and Clark (2010) reported that 90% of the United States population does not suffer any kind of sleep disorder. Still, 40% of individuals report to sleep less than the recommended by the international guidelines (Gallup, 2013). One possibility to explain data is that people are going to bed late, and consequently sleep less than recommended, simply because they delay going to bed. This delay can be regarded from a behavioral perspective, specifically, through the lenses of procrastination.

Procrastination refers to the act of “voluntarily delay an intended course of action despite expecting to be worse off for the delay” (Steel, 2007, p. 66). There is a voluminous corpus of data showing that, overall, procrastination behavior his highly prevalent. For example, Ferrari, Diaz-Morales,

(12)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

O’Callaghan, Díaz, and Argumedo (2007) reported that 20-25% of the general population procrastinates, while 70% of university students consider themselves academic procrastinators (Day, Mensink, & O’Sullivan, 2000; Schouwenburg, 2004). Procrastination behaviors are likely to affect the well-being and health of individuals. For example, procrastination is linked with depression, anxiety (Haycock, McCarthy, & Skay, 1998), and distress (Rice, Richardson, & Clark, 2012). Many people delay the medical doctor appointment, exercising or eating healthily. Besides, individuals can even procrastinate their time to go to bed which may compromise a good night of sleep.

Bedtime procrastination is defined as the act of “going to bed later than intended, without having external reasons for doing so” (Kroese et al., 2014, p. 854). This procrastination behavior requires a delay (“going to bed later than intended”), the absence of a valid reason that could explain the delay (e.g., being sick) and, finally, knowing that their actions will result in negative consequences (Sirois & Pychil, 2016). Procrastination usually involves the delay of a task that individuals find aversive (e.g., writing a report or put the clothes to dry); however, sleeping is not a task that most people consider aversive. In fact, Gershuny (2013) states that enjoyment of individuals for sleeping is above average, which suggests that people do not avoid sleep on purpose.

Bedtime Procrastination is a relatively new concept; thus literature on this topic is limited. Nevertheless, some authors have already put forward possible explanations for the phenomenon. Some relate bedtime procrastination with differences in peoples chronotypes (Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, 2018). For example, Kuhnel, Syrek, and Dreher (2018) found that people with later chronotypes tended to engage more in bedtime procrastination. Still, others examined bedtime procrastination from a motivational perspective, particularly as a self-regulation dysfunction. This perspective interprets bedtime procrastination as a lack of self-control. The rationale is that individuals with low self-control skills will be predisposed to distractions and will not resist distractors, which prevents them from going to bed on time (Kroese et al., 2014; Kroese, Nauts, Kamphorst, Anderson, & De Ridder, 2016). Managing distractors require a high level of self-control, which may not be easy to attain at later time of the day (Kamphorst et al., 2018).

A third family of explanations analyzed bedtime procrastination as a result of the interaction between willpower theories and stress (Bernecker & Job, 2019). These authors argued that the interplay between willpower and daily stress levels can explain bedtime procrastination. Bernecker and Job (2019) found that, in stressful days, adolescents with limited willpower procrastinate more in their bedtime than their counterparts with a non-limited willpower. Lastly, another potential explanation could be general aversion to routines and not specifically to sleep. This aversion can be related to bedtime (e.g., brushing

(13)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

the teeth or changing to the pajamas) or morning (e.g., catch two metros to go to work) routines. Nauts, Kamphorst, Sutu, Poortvliet, and Andersonand (2016) findings show that people who tend to engage in bedtime procrastination rated their bedtime and morning routines as aversive, but there was no relation to sleep aversion.

Purpose of the study

Literature on Sleep Procrastination has typically focused on behaviors preceding bedtime (bedtime procrastination). However, there is also the possibility that individuals delay going to sleep by engaging in other activities while they are already in bed, which we have coined as While-in-bed procrastination. Nowadays, individuals have open access to a vast diversity of entertainment with the potential of keeping them up all night (Kroese et al., 2014). From cellphones to computers and television, the access to different distractions is unlimited, and the enrolment in distractions may cause the postponement of the desired hour to sleep. Additionally, these devices can be used everywhere, including in individuals’ room. In fact, people are using electronic devices in bed, when they should be sleeping, as stated by a study developed by Vernon, Modecki, and Barber (2017) that showed that the use of mobile phones, when lights were off, increased during a three-year period. People can lay in their beds and scroll on their social networks, watch movies on TV or videos on YouTube for hours, losing control of their sleep schedule. This shows that individuals may accomplish their desired bedtime but fail to sleep the desired number of hours; in sum, people may go to bed on time but fail to sleep on time.

Acknowledging the extant research, the main goal of our study was focused on further examining the sleep procrastination process. The present study aimed to analyze the role of While-in-bed Procrastination, along with the Bedtime Procrastination, in the Sleep Procrastination.

Literature has shown that sleep plays a crucial part in brain maturation, particularly when the brain suffers significant changes (Dahl & Lewin 2002; Huber & Bon, 2014;Iglowestein, Jenni, Molinari, & Largo, 2003). Adolescence is a period of transition from childhood to adulthood which leads to transformations at a hormonal, somatic and behavioral level. These processes occur at the same time as maturational sleep mechanisms and other complex processes in our body (Brand & Kirov, 2011). All considered, our study will focus on adolescents (High School students) due to sleep crucial importance in this period.

Method Participants

Participants were recruited through social media (Facebook and Instagram) and personal contacts. A total of 619 participants initiated the questionnaire and 415 completed it (completion rate of

(14)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

67%). Data from 15 participants who scored an abnormal distance from other values in the sample (outliers) were excluded. From the remaining sample (N = 400), 139 (%) were male. The mean age was 16.56 years (SD = 1.07, range 15-20). The majority (n = 156, 39.2%) attended the 12th grade, 129

(32.4%) the 11th grade, and 113 (28.4%) the 10th grade.

Instruments and measures

Sociodemographic Questionnaire. Demographic questions included sex, age, and school year. Additionally, participants were asked about the presence or absence of sleep disorders or problems that could affect their sleep quality/quantity.

Bedtime procrastination Scale. Bedtime procrastination was evaluated through the Bedtime Procrastination Scale developed by Kroese et al. (2014). Originally, this is a nine-item instrument (e.g., “I go to bed later than I had intended”) and items are answered in a five-point Likert-like scale, from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Total scores range between nine and 45 with higher scores indicating more engagement in bedtime procrastination (Cronbach’s alpha = .92). The scale utilize in the current study, adapted to the Portuguese population, was composed with eight items (total scores range from seven to 35) and the Cronbach’s alpha was .85.

While-in-bed Procrastination Scale. To assess whether adolescents delay sleep when they are already in bed, and to learn which activities they engage in before going to sleep, the While-in-bed Procrastination Scale was developed. This instrument is a seven-item scale and some example items are “In bed, before I fall asleep, I watch videos on Youtube” or “In bed, before I fall asleep, I eat snacks (cookies, cereals, milk, chips, chocolate).” Questions were answered on a five-point Likert-like scale, from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Total scores ranged from seven to 35 with higher scores indicating more while-in-bed procrastination (Cronbach’s alpha = .732).

Contextual variables. Variables related to sleep descriptives, family context and routine were included in the questionnaire. These variables were included for a better understanding of the environment and circumstances that the participants live in, and included the following:

Sleep descriptives. Questions regarding sleep habits and tiredness were included in the questionnaire. Participants were asked to indicate their usual waking time and their desired hour to fall asleep. Furthermore, they were asked the quantity of sleep they have, in average, per night and whether they usually feel tired during the day.

Family context. Participants were asked about their parents’ time for arriving home and the number of siblings.

(15)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Routine. Participants were asked about their usual dinner time, their school starting time, and, finally, if they practice any sports (and at what time they usually finish). Additionally, participants were asked to position themselves regarding the following: “Do you consider that you procrastinate before or after going to bed?”.

Procedure

The survey was posted on social media (Instagram, Facebook) and distributed through personal contacts and emails. An informed consent was obtained from each participant prior to their participation. Participation in the study was voluntary, anonymous, and unpaid. Participants who gave their consent to participate filled in the electronic questionnaire that evaluated sociodemographic questions, contextual variables, Bedtime Procrastination, and While-in-bed Procrastination, in this specific order.

Data analysis

The questionnaire included three different sections for analyzes, respectively the sociodemographic questions, the contextual variables (sleep descriptive, family context and routine) and the Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination scales. Context variables were collected through multiple choices answers. To facilitate the analyzes we have categorized the interval variables, with each answer corresponding to a number. For example, the question “usually, your waking time is around” was categorized as 1 (before 7:00 a.m.), 2 (between 7:00 a.m. and 8:00 a.m.), 3 (between 8:00 a.m. and 9:00 a.m.) and 4 (after 9:00 p.m.). The questions with open answer, for example “what time is your sport over?” were grouped according to participants’ answers. Answers were categorized as 1 (before 3:00 p.m.), 2 (3:00 p.m. to 3:59 p.m.), 3 (4:00 p.m. to 4:59 p.m.), 4 (5:00 p.m. to 5:59 p.m.), 5 (6:00 p.m. to 6:59 p.m.), 6 (7:00 p.m. to 7:59 p.m.), 7 (8:00 p.m. to 8:59 p.m.), 8 (9:00 p.m. to 9:59 p.m.) and 9 (10:00 p.m. to 10:59 p.m.). Answers regarding quantity of hours slept per night and number of siblings were not categorized.

The dataset collected were treated and analyzed using the IBM® SPSS® software (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences; IBM, Armonk, NY). Descriptive statistics and frequency analyzes were conducted for all variables in the study. Factorial analysis including the KMO and Bartlett Test, the Communalities test, the total explained variance test, the component matrix and the reliability test were conducted to both scales. Pearson’s correlation was conducted between the Bedtime Procrastination Scale and the While-in-bed procrastination Scale. Lastly, multiple analyses of variance were conducted between sociodemographic variables, the procrastination scales and the context variables.

(16)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY Results

Tables 1, 2, and 3 summarize the descriptive statistics for the Contextual Variables. Specifically, Table 1 displays the descriptive statistics of the sleep descriptives variables. Remarkably, 53.2 % of the participants reported sleeping seven hours or less per night, while only 10.3 % of the sample reported sleeping the recommended number of hours (M = 7.4, SD = 0.9). Additionally, 58.8% reported feeling tired sometimes during the day while 22.8% reported feeling tired almost always. Frequencies for the family context are displayed on Table 2. More than half of the sample reported that their parents arrive home between 5:00 p.m. and 7:00 p.m. Regarding number of siblings, the majority (62.3%) reported to have one sibling, while the minority (1.1%) reported to have four or more siblings. Lastly, Table 3 displays the frequencies of routine related questions. Regarding dinner time, 68% of the participants reported to have dinner between 7:00 p.m. and 8:00 p.m., while only 5.3% of the sample reported to have dinner at 7:00 p.m. Moreover, 68% of the sample have classes starting between 8:00 a.m. and 9:00 a.m. and 85.5% do not share their rooms. Relatively to physical activity, 56.8% reported not playing sports, against 43.2% who practice sports at least once a week. Of the participants who practice sports, 12.1% finish before 06:00 pm, 47.1% between 6:00 p.m. and 8:59 p.m., and finally, 40.8% after 9:00 p.m. Finally, participants were asked whether they procrastinate before going to bed or while-in-bed. The majority (59.5%) reported to procrastinate while-in-bed, weather the rest reported to procrastinate before going to bed.

Table 1

Descriptive statistics of sleep descriptives. Hours of sleep

Frequency %

Less than 5 hours 2 0.6

5-6 hours 52 13.1

6-7 hours 156 39.5

7-8 hours 148 36.5

8-9 hours 36 9.1

9-10 hours 6 1.2

Tiredness during the day

Frequency %

(17)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY Almost never 74 18.5 Almost always 91 22.8 Waking time Frequency % Before 7:00 a.m. 103 25.8

Between 7:00 a.m. and 8:00 a.m. 266 66.5

Between 8:00 a.m. and 9:00 a.m. 23 5.8

After 9 a.m. 8 2

Desired time to fall asleep

Frequency % 9:00 p.m. to 9:59 p.m. 26 6.5 10:00 p.m. to 10:59 p.m. 177 44.3 11:00 p.m. to 11:59 p.m. 153 38.3 12:00 a.m. to 12:59 a.m. 32 8 1:00 a.m. to 3:00 a.m. 6 1.5 Table 2

Descriptive statistics of family context Parents arrival home time

Frequency % 5:00 p.m. 65 16.3 Between 5:00 p.m. and 7:00 p.m. 211 52.8 Between 7:00 p.m. and 9:00 p.m. 101 25.3 After 9:00 p.m. 23 5.8 Number of siblings Frequency % 0 65 16.3 1 249 62.3 2 70 17.5 3 12 3 4 or more 4 1.1

(18)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Table 3

Descriptive statistics of routine. Dinner time

Frequency %

7:00 p.m. 21 5.2

7:00 p.m. and 8:00 p.m. 272 68

After 9 p.m. 107 26.8

School starting time

Frequency %

Between 8:00 a.m. and 10:00 a.m. 393 98.2

Between 10:00 a.m. and 11:00 a.m. 2 0.5

In the afternoon 5 1.3

Room share

Frequency %

Share the room 58 14.5

Sleeps alone 342 85.5

Sports

Frequency %

Yes 173 42.3

No 227 56.8

Time sport is over

Frequency % Before 3:00 p.m. 3 2.1 3:00 p.m. to 3:59 p.m. 1 0.7 4:00 p.m. to 4:59 p.m. 6 14.3 5:00 p.m. to 5:59 p.m. p.m. 7 5 6:00 p.m. to 6:59 p.m. 9 6.4 7:00 p.m. to 7:59 p.m. 14 10 8:00 p.m. to 8:59 p.m. 43 30.7 9:00 p.m. to 9:59 p.m. 32 22.9 10:00 p.m. to 10:59 p.m. 20 14.3

(19)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

11:00 p.m. to 12:59 p.m. 5 3.6

Self-reported procrastination

Frequency %

Before going to bed 162 40.5

While in bed 238 59.5

Sleep procrastination was evaluated via two questionnaires, the Bedtime Procrastination developed by Kroese et al. (2014) and the While-in-bed Procrastination developed for the present study. The first scale has nine items with high inter-item reliability (Cronbach’s α= 0.92). The current study validated the scale to the Portuguese population and was adjusted, ending with eigth items (instead of the original nine) and high inter-item reliability as well (Cronbach’s α= 0.85). Table 4 displays descriptive statistics for this scale. Regarding the second scale, it comprises seven-items about while-in-bed behaviors (Cronbach’s α =.73). The most performed behaviors were “send texts, make videocalls/calls” and “watch videos on YouTube”, while the least performed behavior was “eat snacks (cookies, cereals, milk, chips, chocolate) (see Table 5).

(20)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY Table 4

Bedtime Procrastination Scale frequencies (Portuguese version)

Almost Never Rarely Neutral Frequently Almost Always

Mean (SD) I go to bed later than I had intended 32 (8%) 78 (19.5%) 85 (21.3%) 124 (31%) 81 (20.3%) 3.36 (1.23) I go to bed early if I have to get up early in the morning (R) 58 (15.5%) 98 (24.5%) 120 (30%) 94 (23.5%) 30 (7.5%) 2.85 (1.16) Often I am still doing other things when it is time to go to

bed. 20 (5%) 48 (12%) 64 (16%) 170 (42.5%) 98 (24.5%) 3.70 (1.12)

I easily get distracted by things when I actually would like to

go to bed. 43 (10.8%) 60 (15%) 95 (23.8%) 129 (32.3%) 73 (18.3%) 3.32 (1.24)

I do not go to bed on time. 43 (10.8%) 61 (15.3%) 104 (26 %) 113 (28.2%) 79 (19.8%) 3.31 (1.25)

I have a regular bedtime which I keep to (R). 42 (10.5%) 94 (23.5%) 89 (22.3%) 84 (21%) 91 (22.8%) 3.22 (1.32) I want to go to bed on time but I just don’t. 76 (19%) 89 (22.3%) 85 (21.3%) 89 (22.3%) 61 (15.3%) 2.93 (1.347) I can easily stop with my activities when it is time to go to

(21)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY Table 5

While-in-bed Procrastination Scale frequencies

Almost Never Rarely Neutral Frequently Almost

Always Mean (SD)

Watch videos on youtube 69 (17.3%) 58 (14.5%) 46 (11.5%) 129 (32.3%) 98 (24.5%) 3.32 (1.43)

Watch TV 142 (35.5%) 82 (20.5%) 63 (15.8%) 59 (14.8%) 54 (13.5%) 2.5 (1.44)

Watch movies and series 89 (22.3%) 67 (16.8%) 66 (16.5%) 100 (25%) 78 (19.5%) 3.03 (1.45)

Listen to music 76 (19%) 66 (16.5%) 48 (12%) 100 (25%) 110 (27.5%) 3.26 (1.49)

Send texts, make videocalls/calls 59 (14.8%) 46 (11.5%) 60 (15%) 110 (27.5%) 125 (31.3%) 3.49 (1.41) Play games (tablet, computer, cellphone) 158 (39.5%) 69 (17.3%) 68 (17%) 55 (13.8%) 50 (12.5 %) 2.43 (1.44) Eat snacks (cookies, cereals, milk, chips, chocolate) 185 (46.3%) 68 (17%) 58 (14.5%) 52 (13%) 37 (9.3%) 2.22 (1.38)

(22)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Pearson’s correlation was calculated between Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination (see Table 6). This allows to test whether the scales are measuring the same construct, if they are highly correlated, or whether they are measuring different constructs, if they are poorly or not correlated. Data shows that correlation between scales is low (r = .158**), which supports the contention that Sleep Procrastination may be composed by two aspects: Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination.

Table 6

Bedtime Procrastination Scale and While-in-bed Procrastination Scale correlation

Bedtime Procrastination While-in-bed Procrastination

Bedtime Procrastination - .158**

While-in-bed Procrastination .158** -

Note. **The correlation is significant in a 0.01 level, two tailed.

Analyses of Variance were performed for some contextual and sociodemographic variables with Bedtime Procrastination (M = 3.2, SD = 0.9) and While-in-bed Procrastination (M = 2.9, SD = 0.9) (see Table 7). Regarding Bedtime Procrastination, results show that there were significant differences in Bedtime Procrastination regarding waking time and dinner time, F(3.396) = 4.63, p < .005 and F(2.397) = 7.13, p < .005, respectively. Gabriel's Post-Hoc Test revealed that participants reporting waking up after 9:00 a.m. reported more bedtime procrastination compared to participants reporting waking up before 7:00 a.m. and between 7:00 a.m. and 8:00 a.m. Regarding dinner time, participants reporting having dinner after 9:00 p.m. report more bedtime procrastination compared to participants reporting having dinner at 7:00 p.m. and between 7:00 p.m. and 8:00 p.m.

Regarding While-in-bed Procrastination, the scenario alters. Results showed that there are significant differences regarding sex, F(1, 398) = 11.73, p < .005, desired sleep time, F(4, 389) = 5.999, p < .001, and dinner time, F(2, 397) = 6.181, p < .005. Gabriel's Post-Hoc Test showed that male participants reported more While-in-bed Procrastination compared to female participants. Regarding desired sleep time, adolescents who reported wanting to sleep between 1:00 a.m. and 3:00 a.m. reported more While-in-bed Procrastination than adolescents who reported wanting to sleep between 9:00 p.m. and 9:59 p.m., 10:00 p.m. and 10:59 p.m., 11:00 p.m. and 11:59 p.m. and, 12:00 a.m. and 00:59 a.m.. Lastly, the Post-Hoc Test regarding the variable Dinner time showed that there are differences between the group of participants who report having dinner at 7:00 p.m. and the other two groups.

(23)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Accordingly, participants who reported having dinner at 7:00 p.m. tended to engage more in While-in-bed Procrastination than participants who reported having dinner between 7:00 p.m. and 8:00 p.m. and after 9:00 p.m.

Discussion

Sleep plays a vital role in our health. Sleeping the ideal number of hours of sleep benefits cognition, mental and physical health, conveying wellbeing (Strazzullo, Cairella, Garbagnati, Cappuccio, & Scalfi, 2010; Curcio, Ferrara, & De Gennaro, 2006; Magee, Caputi, & Iverson, 2010; Mander, Santhanam, Saletin, & Walker, 2011; Van der Helm, Gujar, & Walker, 2010). The current study found that adolescents, who need a good sleep hygiene for their normative development, are sleeping two hours less per night than the recommended number of hours. This finding, despite worrying, is consistent with previous literature (Bernecker & Job, 2019; Woods & Scott 2016). Amidst this scenario, the purpose of this paper was to explore the possibility that sleep insufficiency may be due to procrastination of sleep. Particularly, we looked at Sleep Procrastination as a broader phenomenon, aiming to examine whether there is evidence that this construct consists of two facets: Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination.

To measure Bedtime Procrastination, the Bedtime Procrastination Scale, developed by Kroese et al. (2014), was adapted and validated for the Portuguese population. The authors provided additional information about the scale results (frequencies of the items), and it can be concluded that the Portuguese

Table 7 Analyses of variance Bedtime Procrastination While-in-bed Procrastination Sex F (1, 398) F (1, 398) **

Desired hour to fall asleep F (4, 389) F (4, 389) **

Usual dinner time F (2, 397) ** F (2, 397) *

Usual waking time F (3, 396) * F (3, 396)

School hour F (2, 397) F (2, 397)

School year F (2, 395) F (2, 395)

Self-reported Procrastination F (1, 398) F (1, 398)

Number of siblings F (6, 393) F (6, 393)

(24)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

version follows the same tendency as the original scale. The means of the items were particularly similar (the maximum variation was one point in one item) as well as the general means of each scale. Regarding the concept of While-in-bed Procrastination, we developed a scale to capture this phenomenon. The correlation between both scales was low (p = .158**), suggesting that these instruments are measuring different phenomena, thus supporting our assertion that Sleep Procrastination may comprise two aspects: procrastination behaviors before going to bed and after going to bed.

Interestingly, the behaviours individuals reported engaging more while in bed were to watch videos on YouTube, watch movies, listen to music and send texts. These findings are consistent with those reported by the National Sleep Foundation (2006). This report indicated that all adolescents used at least one electronic device in bed, particularly: 57% television, 90% music players, 43% computers, and 64% phones. Additionally, the study by Calamaro, Mason, and Ratcliffe (2009) reported that adolescents were sending texts, making phone calls, playing computer games or were online after 9:00 p.m.; and the study by Van den Bulck (2007) using a Belgian sample reported that 62% of adolescents used their phone in bed, with the lights off, and consequently reported more tiredness during the following day.

Lastly, the present study allowed to examine differences in contextual variables as a function of type of procrastination, Bedtime or While-in-bed. Whereas Bedtime Procrastination was related to the context variables waking and dinner time, While-in-bed Procrastination was linked to sex, desired time to sleep, and dinner time. This pattern seems to indicate three essential aspects: sex, ambient control, and sleep goals. More specifically, it seems that sex, ambient control (dinner time) and sleep goals (desired sleep and wake hour) predicted distinctly the facet of Sleep procrastination (Bedtime or While-in-bed procrastination). According to the literature, men show lower self-regulatory skills than women and, therefore, engage further in procrastination (Özer, Demir, & Ferrari, 2009; Steel & Ferrari, 2012). However, we have not found potential explanations in the literature regarding the remaining results.

These results encompass extreme importance since this research is the first in the literature analyzing Sleep Procrastination from a different standpoint. The analysis of Sleep Procrastination as having two facets, Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination, is likely to disclose new research opportunities and contribute to further accurate intervention. Current literature in the Sleep Procrastination domain remains in the path of discovering the underlying mechanisms behind the phenomenon, but current findings on sleep insufficiency have already showed the relevance of the phenomenon to our health and well-being. Consequently, a suitable intervention is needed, likewise other domains of procrastination, and having the knowledge that it could comprise two types of procrastinators will allow the development of more effective interventions. More precisely, an intervention design for

(25)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

intervene in the behaviors participants enroll in before going to bed (bedtime procrastinators) should be accomplished as well as the development of an intervention that acts in the behaviors of participants when they are in their beds (while-in-bed procrastinators), having with basis the existing and further research to settle strategies to help individuals accomplish a proper sleep routine. In fact, a study made by Kroese et al. (2014) showed a high correlation between Bedtime Procrastination and general procrastination, which is linked to a low self-regulation, being a possible explanation for the phenomenon. However, another study found that Bedtime Procrastination was a result of biological individual characteristics, opposing to the idea of the phenomenon being a result of a lack of self-regulatory resources (Kühnel et al., 2018). The literature about this area remains scarce and inconsistent, and we suggest further research and the need to include both Bedtime Procrastination and While-in-bed Procrastination.

Limitations and future studies

The current research has methodological constraints that should be considered when interpreting the results. The reader should bear in mind that the nature of this study was correlational and does not provide evidence about the causality of the effects. Additionally, data was collected online and through a self-report questionnaire. Thus, we suggest future studies to apply a momentary form of evaluation (e.g., sleep diaries), which would add additional validity to the research. Nonetheless, we have no reasons to believe that these limitations bring prejudice to the study, since the reliability of the measures was high.

Hence, this research provided an important opportunity to advance the understanding of Sleep Procrastination. Consequently, having into account that this study opened new research possibilities, future research may consider to develop further research and educational interventions on the underlying mechanisms of Sleep Procrastination. Furthermore, it would be of interest to relate Sleep Procrastinators with other health procrastination behaviors (e.g., medical and healthy eating procrastination). These findings are expected to help understand why people keep engage in behaviors likely to undermine their own health and well-being.

Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to undertake a different direction in Sleep Procrastination, adding a new perspective to the literature. The results suggest that assessing Sleep Procrastination solely focused on procrastinating behaviours before going to bed is not enough to fully understand the phenomenon. Delaying sleep while-in-bed is a common reality and need to be acknowledged as part of the Sleep Procrastination process. Hence, there are several important aspects in which this study makes an original contribution. As described above, sleep insufficiency is a subject of

(26)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

extreme importance and affects a significant size of the general population; so, hopefully this study will contribute to its improvement.

(27)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY References

Basch, C. E., Basch, C. H., Ruggles, K. V., & Rajan, S. (2014). Prevalence of Sleep Duration on an Average School Night Among 4 Nationally Representative Successive Samples of American High School Students, 2007-2013. Preventing Chronic Disease, 11. doi:10.5888/pcd11.140383

Belenky, G., Wesensten, N. J., Thorne, D. R., Thomas, M. L., Sing, H. C., Redmond, D. P., … Balkin, T. J. (2003). Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: A sleep dose-response study. Journal of Sleep Research, 12, 1-12. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.x

Bernecker, K., & Job, V. (2019). Too exhausted to go to bed: Implicit theories about willpower and stress predict bedtime procrastination. British Journal of Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12382

Brand, S., & Kirov, R. (2011). Sleep and its importance in adolescence and in common adolescent somatic and psychiatric conditions. International Journal of General Medicine, 4, 425-442. doi:10.2147/ijgm.s11557 Van den Bulck J. (2007). Adolescent use of mobile phones for calling and for sending text messages after lights

out: Results from a prospective cohort study with a one-year follow-up. Sleep, 30(9), 1220-1223. doi:10.1093/sleep/30.9.1220

Buxton, O. M., & Marcelli, E. (2010). Short and long sleep are positively associated with obesity, diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease among adults in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 71(5), 1027-1036. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.05.041

Calamaro, C. J., Mason, T. B. A., & Ratcliffe, S. J. (2009). Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics, 123(6), e1005-e1010. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-3641

Centre for Disease Control and Prevention (2014). Insufficient sleep is a public health epidemic. Available from

www.cdc.gov/features/dssleep/

Chen, M. C., Burley, H. W., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Reduced sleep quality in healthy girls at risk for depression. Journal of Sleep Research, 21(1), 68-72. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00934.x

Clements Z, Fabó D, Halász P. (2005). Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience, 132(2), 529-535. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.011

Conklin, A.I., Yao, C.A. & Richardson, C.G. (2018). Chronic sleep deprivation and gender-specific risk of depression in adolescents: a prospective population-based study. BMC Public Health, 18(1), 724. doi:10.1186/s12889-018-5656-6

Connor, J., Norton, R., Ameratunga, S., Robinson, E., Civil, I., Dunn, R., … Jackson, R. (2002). Driver sleepiness and risk of serious injury to car occupants: Population based case control study. British Medical Journal, 324(7346), 1125-1128.

Curcio, G., Ferrara, M., & De Gennaro, L. (2006). Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10(5), 323-337. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.11.001

Dahl, R. E., & Lewin, D. S. (2002). Pathways to adolescent health sleep regulation and behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31(6), 175-184. doi:10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00506-2 37

Day, V., Mensink, D., & O’Sullivan, M. (2014). Patterns of Academic Procrastination. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 30(2), 120-134. doi:10.1080/10790195.2000.10850090

Diekelmann S, Born J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2),114-126. doi:10.1038/nrn2762

Fallone, G., Owens, J.A., Deane, J. (2002). Sleepiness in children and adolescents: Clinical implications. Sleep Medicine Reviews ,6(4),287-306. doi:10.1053/smrv.2001.0192

Ferrari, J. R., Díaz-Morales, J. F., O’Callaghan, J., Díaz, K., & Argumedo, D. (2007). Frequent behavioral delay tendencies by adults: International prevalence rates of chronic procrastination. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(4), 458-464. doi:10.1177/0022022107302314

(28)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Gallup. (2013). In U.S., 40% Get Less Than Recommended Amount of Sleep. Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/poll/166553/less-recommended-amount-sleep.aspx

Gershuny, J. (2013). National utility: Measuring the enjoyment of activities. European Sociological Review, 29(5), 996-1009. doi:10.1093/esr/jcs077

Gottlieb, D. J., Redline, S., Nieto, F. J., Baldwin, C. M., Newman, A. B., Resnick, H. E., & Punjabi, N. M. (2006). Association of usual sleep duration with hypertension: The Sleep Heart Health Study. Sleep, 29(8), doi:10.1093/sleep/29.8.1009

Gradisar, M., Gardner, G., & Dohnt, H. (2010). Recent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during adolescence: A review and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleep. Sleep Medicine, 12(2), 110-118. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.008

Gujar, N., McDonald, S.A., Nishida, M., & Walker, M.P. (2011). A role for rem sleep in recalibrating the sensitivity of the human brain to specific emotions. Cerebral Cortex, 21(1), 115-123. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq064 Gupta, N. K., Mueller, W. H., Chan, W., & Meininger, J. C. (2002). Is obesity associated with poor sleep quality

in adolescents? American Journal of Human Biology, 14(6), 762–768. doi:10.1002/ajhb.10093 url to share this paper:

Haycock, L. A., McCarthy, P., & Skay, C. L. (1998). Procrastination in college students: The role of self-efficacy and anxiety. Journal of Counseling and Development, 76(3), 317-324. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.1998.tb02548.x

Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., … Adams Hillard, P. J. (2015). National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: Methodology and results summary. Sleep Health, 1(1), 40-43. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.010

Hirshkowitz, M., Whiton, K., Albert, S. M., Alessi, C., Bruni, O., DonCarlos, L., … Ware, J. C. (2015). National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: Final report. Sleep Health, 1(4), 233-243. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004

Holliday, E.G., Magee, C.A., Kritharides, L., Banks, E., & Attia, J. (2013). Short sleep duration is associated with risk of future diabetes but not cardiovascular disease: A prospective study and meta analysis. PLOSOne, 8(11), e82305. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082305

Huber, R., & Born, J., 2014. Sleep, synaptic connectivity, and hippocampal memory during early development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 141-152. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.005

Iglowstein, I., Jenni, O.G., Molinari, L., &Largo, R.H. (2003). Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics, 111(2), 302-307. doi:10.1542/peds.111.2.302 Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, R. (2018). Insufficient Sleep among adolescents: The role of bedtime procrastination,

chronotype and autonomous vs. controlled motivational regulations. Current Psychology. doi:10.1007/s12144-018-9825-7

Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, R. (2018). Self-regulation and bedtime procrastination: The role of self-regulation skills and chronotype. Personality and Individual Differences, 128, 10-15. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.015 Kamphorst, B. A., Nauts, S., De Ridder, D.T.D., & Anderson, J.H. (2018). Too Depleted to Turn In: The Relevance

of End-of-the-Day Resource Depletion for Reducing Bedtime Procrastination. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 252. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00252

Kirov, R., Uebel, H., Albrecht, B., Banaschewski, T., & Rothenberger, A. (2011). Two faces of REM sleep in normal and psychopathological development. European Psychiatry, 26(1), 422-423. doi:10.1016/s0924-9338(11)72130-7

Knutson, K. L., Van Cauter, E., Rathouz, P. J., Yan, L. L., Hulley, S. B., Liu K., & Lauderdale, D. S. (2009). Association between sleep and blood pressure in midlife: The CARDIA sleep study. Archives Internal Medicine, 169(11), 1055-1061. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2009.119

Kroese, F. M., De Ridder, D. T. D., Evers, C., & Adriaanse, M. A. (2014). Bedtime procrastination: Introducing a new area of procrastination. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00611

(29)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Kroese, F. M., Evers, C., Adriaanse, M. A., & de Ridder, D. T. (2014). Bedtime procrastination: A self-regulation perspective on sleep insufficiency in the general population. Journal of Health Psychology, 21(5), 853-862. doi:10.1177/1359105314540014

Krueger, J. M., Rector, D. M., Roy, S., Van Dongen, H. P. A., Belenky, G., & Panksepp, J. (2008). Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 910-919. doi:10.1038/nrn2521

Krueger, P. M., & Friedman, E. M. (2009). Sleep Duration in the United States: A Cross-sectional Population-based Study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 169(9), 1052–1063. doi:10.1093/aje/kwp023 Kühnel, J., Syrek, C. J., & Dreher, A. (2018). Why don’t you go to bed on time? A daily diary study on the

relationships between chronotype, self-control resources and the phenomenon of bedtime procrastination. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00077

Lofthouse, N., Gilchrist, R., & Splaingard, M. (2009). Mood-related sleep problems in children and adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 18(4), 893-916. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2009.04.007

Magee, C. A., Caputi, P., & Iverson, D. C. (2010). Is sleep duration associated with obesity in older Australian adults? Journal of Aging and Health, 22(8), 1235-1255. doi:10.1177/0898264310372780

Mander, B. A., Santhanam, S., Saletin, J. M., & Walker, M. P. (2011). Wake deterioration and sleep restoration of human learning. Current Biology, 21(5), R183-R184. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.019

Maquet P. (2001). The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science, 294(5544), 1048-1052. doi:10.1126/science.1062856

McNamara, P., McLaren, D., Smith, D., Brown, A., & Stickgold, R. (2005). A “Jekyll and Hyde” within: aggressive versus friendly interactions in REM and non-REM dreams. Psychological Science, 16(2), 130-136. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00793.x

Meijer, A.M., Habekothé, H.T., & Van Den Wittenboer, G.L. (2000). Time in bed, quality of sleep and school functioning of children. Journal of Sleep Research, 9(2), 145-153.

Ming, X., Koransky, R., Kang, V., Buchman, S., Sarris, C. E., & Wagner, G. C. (2011). Sleep insufficiency, sleep health problems and performance in high school students. Clinical Medicine Insights: Circulatory, Respiratory and Pulmonary Medicine, 5, 71-19. doi:10.4137/CCRPM.S7955

Moore, M., & Meltzer, L. J. (2008). The sleepy adolescent: Causes and consequences of sleepiness in teens. Pediatric Respiratory Reviews, 9(2), 114-121. doi:10.1016/j.prrv.2008.01.001

Nauts, S.; Kamphorst, B.A.; Sutu, Andreea E.; Poortvliet, Rune; Anderson, J.H. (2016) The European Health Psychologist, 18(2), 80 – 85.

National Center for Statistics and Analysis. (2017, October). Drowsy Driving 2015 (Crash•Stats Brief Statistical Summary. Report No. DOT HS 812 446). Washington, DC, US: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration.

Nedeltcheva, A. V., Kilkus, J. M., Imperial, J., Schoeller, D. A., & Penev, P.D. (2010). Insufficient sleep Undermines Dietary Efforts to Reduce adiposity. Annals of Internal Medicine, 153(7), 435-441. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-153-7-201010050-00006

Nishida, M., Pearsall, J., Buckner, R. L., & Walker, M. P. (2008). REM sleep, prefrontal theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cerebral Cortex, 19(5), 1158-1166. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn155

Nishiura, C., & Hashimoto, H. (2010). A 4-year study of the association between short sleep duration and change in body mass index in Japanese male workers. Journal of Epidemiology, 20(5), 385-390. doi:10.2188/jea.je20100019

Ohkuma, T., Fujii, H., Iwase, M., Kikuchi, Y., Ogata, S., Idewaki, Y., … Kitazono, T. (2012). Impact of sleep duration on obesity and the glycemic level in patients with type 2 diabetes: The Fukuoka Diabetes Registry. Diabetes Care, 36(3), 611-617. doi:10.2337/dc12-0904

Owens, J., & Adolescent Sleep Working Group. (2014). Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: an update on causes and consequences. Pediatrics, 134(3), e921-e932. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-1696

(30)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Özer, B. U., Demir, A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2009). Exploring academic procrastination among Turkish students: Possible gender differences in prevalence and reasons. The Journal of Social Psychology, 149(2), 241-257. doi:10.3200/socp.149.2.241-257

Petrov, M. E. R., Kim, Y., Lauderdale, D., Lewis, C. E., Reis, J. P., Carnethon, M. R., … Glasser, S. J. (2013). Longitudinal associations between objective sleep and lipids: The CARDIA study. Sleep, 36(11), 1587-1595. doi:10.5665/sleep.3104

Pychyl, T. A., & Sirois, F. M. (2016). Procrastination, emotion regulation, and well-being. In F. M. Sirois & T. A. Pychyl (Eds.), Procrastination, health, and well-being (pp. 163-188). San Diego, CA, US: Elsevier Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-802862-9.00008-6

Ram, S., Seirawan, H., Kumar, S.K., & Clark, G. T. (2010). Prevalence and impact of sleep disorders and sleep habits in the United States. Sleep Breath. 14(1), 63-70. doi:10.1007/s11325-009-0281-3

Rice, K. G., Richardson, C. M. E., & Clark, D. (2012). Perfectionism, procrastination, and psychological distress. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59(2), 288-302. doi:10.1037/a0026643

Roane, B.M., & Taylor, J.D. (2008). Adolescent insomnia as a risk factor for early adult depression and substance abuse. Sleep, 31(10), 1351-1356. doi:10.5665/sleep/31.10.1351

Roberts, R. E., Roberts, C. R., & Duong, H. T. (2009). Sleepless in adolescence: Prospective data on sleep deprivation, health and functioning. Journal of Adolescence, 32(5), 1045-1057. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.03.007

Schabus, M., Hödlmoser, K., Gruber, G., Sauter, C., Anderer, P., Klösch, G., … Zeitlhofer, J. (2006). Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(7), 1738-1746. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04694.x

Schouwenburg, H. C., Lay, C. H., Pychyl, T. A., & Ferrari, J. R. (Eds.). (2004). Counseling the procrastinator in academic settings. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

Sheehan, C. M., Frochen, S. E., Walsemann, K. M., & Ailshire, J. A. (2018). Are U.S. adults reporting less sleep?: Findings from sleep duration trends in the national health interview survey. Sleep, 42(2). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsy221

Shin, H. Y., Kang, G., Kim, S.-W., Kim, J.-M., Yoon, J.-S., & Shin, I.-S. (2016). Associations between sleep duration and abnormal serum lipid levels: Data from the Korean national health and nutrition examination survey (KNHANES). Sleep Medicine, 24, 119-123. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2016.05.012

Shochat, T., Cohen-Zion, M., & Tzischinsky, O. (2014). Functional consequences of inadequate sleep in adolescents: Asystematic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 18(1), 75-87. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2013.03.005

Siegel, J. M. (2005). Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature, 437(7063), 1264-1271. doi:10.1038/nature04285

Siegel, J. M. (2009). Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(10), 747-753. doi:10.1038/nrn2697

Spiegel, K., Tasali, E., Penev, P., & Van Cauter, E. (2004). Brief communication: Sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and increased hunger and appetite. Annals of Internal Medicine, 141(11), 846-850. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2013.03.005.

Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65-94. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65

Steel, P., & Ferrari, J. (2012). Sex, education and procrastination: An epidemiological study of procrastinators’ characteristics from a global sample. European Journal of Personality, 27(1), 51-58. doi:10.1002/per.1851

Strazzullo, P., D’Elia, L., Cairella, G., Garbagnati, F., Cappuccio, F. P., & Scalfi, L. (2010). Excess body weight

and incidence of stroke: Meta-analysis of prospective studies with 2 million participants. Stroke, 41(5), e418-e426. doi:10.1161/strokeaha.109.576967

(31)

SLEEP PROCRASTINATION: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY

Strine, T. W., & Chapman, D. P. (2005). Associations of frequent sleep insufficiency with health-related quality of life and health behaviors. Sleep Medicine, 6, 23-27. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2004.06.003

The National Sleep Foundation (2005). Sleep in America poll - adult sleep habits and styles. Sleep Health, 1(2), e4. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2015.04.004

The National Sleep Foundation (2006). Sleep in America poll - teens and sleep. Sleep Health, 1(2), e5. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2015.04.005

Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10(1), 49-62. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002

Van der Helm, E., Gujar, N., & Walker, M. P. (2010). Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep, 33(3), 335-342. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.3.335

Vernon, L., Modecki, K. L., & Barber, B. L. (2017). Mobile phones in the bedroom: Trajectories of sleep habits and subsequent adolescent psychosocial development. Child Development, 89(1), 66-77. doi:10.1111/cdev.12836

Walker, M.P. (2009). The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156(1), 168-197. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x

Walker, M.P., & Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 139-166. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070307

Watanabe, M., Kikuchi, H., Tanaka, K., & Takahashi, M. (2010). Association of short sleep duration with weight gain and obesity at 1-year follow-up: A large-scale prospective study. Sleep, 33(2), 161-167. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.2.161

Woods, H. C., & Scott, H. (2016). #Sleepyteens: Social media use in adolescence is associated with poor sleep quality, anxiety, depression and low self-esteem. Journal of Adolescence, 51, 41-49. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.008

Imagem

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of sleep descriptives ________________________________ 14  Table 2 Descriptive statistics of family context ___________________________________ 15  Table 3 Descriptive statistics of routine
Table 7  Analyses of variance  Bedtime  Procrastination  While-in-bed  Procrastination   Sex  F (1, 398)  F (1, 398) **

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Este estudo tem como objetivo caracterizar como os projetos de cinema e educação funcionam no seu dia-a-dia; identificar que tipo de pedagogias e metodologias são utilizadas; saber

The items, presented in different formats (as described below), approach varied themes among which we will specially emphasize one: the use of journals, including the type of

Results: High prevalence of adolescents aged between 15 and 17 years old, born and coming from urban areas, with no kinship to the newborn’s father, with no history of abortions,

O objetivo desse trabalho é incluir, para a fauna brasileira, mais essa espécie de Membracidae, cujos exemplares foram coletados no Estado de São Paulo, em

Parece haver consenso suficiente na literatura para referir que a confiança é um conceito fundamental nas relações entre empresas (Ganesan, 1994; Wilson, 1995). O Compromisso

The purpose of this study was to analyze the relationship between self-efficacy for professional choice and career adaptability in high school students, as well as to verify

It is worth noting a survey of 3,427 high school students in Croatia who indicated that good self-perception of health outcomes was significantly associated

(2013) conducted a study with 357 high-school and elementary-school teachers from the public network of São Luís, Maranhão, using the Lipp’s Stress Symptoms Inventory for