• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Emergent Multidimensional Problem-Solving Framework

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2023

Share "Emergent Multidimensional Problem-Solving Framework"

Copied!
45
0
0

Texto

ABSTRACT: This article describes the problem-solving behavior of twelve mathematicians as they complete four mathematical tasks. The framework also characterizes several problem-solving features (resources, affect, heuristics, and monitoring) and describes their role and significance during each of the problem-solving phases. Other studies have revealed the influence of different affective dimensions (e.g., beliefs, attitudes, and emotions) on the problem-solving process.

He described the problem-solving process as a linear progression from one stage to another and advocated that when a problem is solved, Following Schoenfeld (1992, p. 355), we will use the term monitoring to mean the mental actions involved in reflecting on the effectiveness of the problem-solving process and products. We define resources as conceptual understandings, knowledge, facts, and procedures used when solving problems.

However, our initial analysis using our problem-solving taxonomy revealed that it was limited in its ability to characterize some of the problem-solving behaviors we were observing -. eg, interactions between different elements in the taxonomy. The CEL effort is made to construct logically connected statements CM Metacognitive Behaviors during problem solving.

THE STUDY 1 Methods

In particular, we noted that our framework was limited in its ability to indicate specific interactions between the problem-solving process and aspects of subjects' cognitive processes, metacognitive behaviors, and emotional responses. We also observed that when thinking about different approaches to a solution during the planning phase of the problem-solving process, mathematicians sometimes engaged in a hypothesis-idea-verify cycle (Figure 1). We also sought to better understand the nature of cognitive and metacognitive processes and their interaction with other aspects of problem solving identified in our initial taxonomy.

We also reviewed all coded transcripts to identify and characterize the most common ways in which resources, heuristics, affect, and control were displayed during each of the problem-solving phases. It was at this point that we also noticed patterns in the way different control mechanisms were used during troubleshooting. We further observed that these behaviors, reasoning patterns, and knowledge influenced the mathematicians' success in solving problems.

RESULTS

After marking the height of the triangle with x, he verified that the area of ​​the black triangle is x2 (9,10). His next expression (11), along with his next statement (12), indicated that he quickly realized that the area of ​​the white triangle could also represent x2. Once he could visualize the problem, he proceeded to make a drawing of the situation (drawing a square and labeling each side with length 3 [2]).

After drawing an illustration of folded paper, his verbalizations indicated that he imagined folding corner A onto the diagonal of the square (3). He then labeled each of the three parts of his figure with an A (ie, each triangle formed by the fold and the strip representing the difference between the original square and the square formed by the fold). He then made another conjecture that led to his conclusion that the area of ​​a triangle.

His re-engagement again led to his attempt to make sense of the problem and restore known facts (26–30). He then confidently stated that the area of ​​the black triangle (back side of what is folded over) is 1 (31). After again determining that the side of the smaller square is x, and observing that the area of ​​this square is 2, he came to the conclusion (using his knowledge of the relationship between the area and side of a square to use) that the length of x must be equal to 2 (32, 33).

Marco initially read the problem and made several attempts to understand it. Once he had decided on a strategy, he seemed to use his knowledge of basic geometric properties to determine how to express the areas of the two equal figures algebraically (13–15). His decision to proceed was based on his expressed desire to try the other problems within the limited time frame of the interview.

A was folded over; He then moved the corner up and down diagonally until he finally came to a point that seemed to meet the requirements of the problem (the area was. Expression of Intimacy (15) So that triangle becomes half of the square, so Cycles forward check (20) Now I need to go back and find the length of the piece of the triangle so I know how far it is from the fold line.

TABLE III
TABLE III

AN EMERGING FRAMEWORK

At this point, we also observed that as the mathematicians moved toward a solution, four distinct phases (characterized by distinct shifts in the mathematicians' cognitive behavior) uniformly emerged in their problem-solving processes (Table III, Table IV, Table V; Column 3). ). This analysis also revealed that reflections on and decisions about the general problem-solving approach typically occurred just before the mathematician entered the execution phase of the problem-solving process (e.g., Table V, line 13). Their exhibited behavior included spoken reflections on the reasonableness of the solution and written calculations.

When the checking phase resulted in a rejection of the solution, the solver returned to the planning phase and repeated the cycle (Figure 1). When the check resulted in an acceptance of the solution, the subject continued in another plan-execute-check cycle until the problem was completed. These reflections occurred frequently during each of the four stages of problem solving and seemed to move the mathematicians' thinking and output in generally productive directions.

In further analysis of the transcripts, we found that metacognitive actions in each problem-solving phase were best characterized as actions. We also saw that certain monitoring behaviors sometimes influenced mathematicians' strategic control decisions, which can be captured by examining their transcripts as they move through problem-solving cycles. This involved our re-analysis of each coded transcript to identify common uses of resources (knowledge, facts, procedures) and heuristics (constructing a diagram, attempting a parallel problem, etc.) and consistent displays of affect (pleasure, pride, frustration, mathematical integrity) and monitoring (reflection on the effectiveness and efficiency of the resolution process) for each phase of the problem resolution.

The interactions we observed between these attributes led us to develop more detailed descriptions of the role that each key problem-solving attribute appears to play in each problem-solving phase (Table VII, columns). The Multidimensional Problem Solving Framework provides a detailed characterization of phases and cycles that occurred consistently in the problem-solving process, as well as the specific behaviors our twelve mathematicians exhibited as they attempted to solve four of them. The framework describes how resources and heuristics interacted with the overall behavior exhibited during the four phases of problem solving (orient, plan, execute, and control).

The framework further illustrates the cyclical nature of the problem-solving process and the strategic points at which overall control influenced mathematicians' problem-solving decisions and actions. The cells on the right contain a description of the primary role of four problem-solving traits (i.e., resources, heuristics, affect, and monitoring) during that problem-solving phase. During the orientation phase, the mathematicians initially made intensive efforts to understand the information in the problem.

Table VI
Table VI

CONCLUDING REMARKS

1992; DeFranco, 1996), previous studies have not provided a detailed characterization of the interplay between metacognition and conceptual knowledge. Among the mathematicians we studied, well-connected conceptual knowledge appeared to influence all phases of the problem-solving process (orienting, planning, execution, and verification). The many dimensions and diverse components of the MPS framework suggest that learning how to become an effective problem solver requires the development and coordination of a large reservoir of patterns of thinking, knowledge, and behavior, and the effective management of resources and emotional responses that occur during the problem-solving process, as well as a lot of practice and experience.

We believe that our study has brought additional clarity to the problem-solving process and successfully brought together much of the existing problem-solving literature. Make a rough sketch of the graph of the height as a function of the amount of water in the bottle. Considers the efficiency and effectiveness of various methods Considers the effectiveness and efficiency of cognitive activities Strives to remain mentally engaged.

Metacognition and Mathematical Intimacy in Expert Problem Solvers', in the 24th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Haifa, Israel, pp. A.: 1998, 'Mathematical intimacy: local affect in high-performance problem solvers', in Twentieth Annual Conference of the North American Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Eric Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics and Environmental Education, pp. A.: 1997, 'The affective domain in mathematical problem solving', in the twenty-first annual meeting of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Lahti, Finland, University of Helsinki, pp.

Mathematical concepts, facts and algorithms were obtained when trying to make sense of the problem. Motivation to make sense of the problem was influenced by their strong curiosity and high interest. Conceptual concepts and numerical intuitions were used to reflect on the reasonableness of the solution's progress and products when solution statements were constructed.

Resources, including well-connected conceptual knowledge, informed the solver about the reasonableness or correctness of the solution reached. Computational and algorithmic shortcuts were used to check the correctness of the answers and determine the reasonableness of the calculations. Reflections on the efficiency, correctness and aesthetic quality of the solution provided useful feedback to the solver.

Imagem

TABLE III
TABLE IV
Table VI

Referências

Documentos relacionados

A língua traduz a cultura de um povo, bem como determina os parâmetros linguísticos de uma sociedade; a cultura, por sua vez, influencia a língua por meio do