• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Consumer innovativeness as a prerequisite of private label success in emerging markets

5. Discussion & Implications

Finally, mediation analysis produced significant results for all four steps of the Baron and Kenny (1986) procedure, indicating complementary mediation (see Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010).

The indirect effect accounted for about 55% of the total effect and a Sobel test showed that it was statistically significant (z = 4.35, p < .001). Full details for the mediation analysis are provided in the figure below.

incongruity in various situations, results are inconclusive and, with a few exceptions, only limited attention has been given to the ad – brand relationship (Halkias & Kokkinaki, 2011a, 2011b;

Noseworthy et al., 2010). Besides, most of the relevant studies have used a dichotomous operationalization of (in)congruity that merely distinguishes between match and mismatch without taking into consideration differences in the degree of incongruity. This prevents comparisons across studies, since different degrees of discrepancy (from a slight to severe) fall under the term

“incongruent”, although they may essentially refer to a totally different case. Some of the empirical inconsistencies found in the literature may be attributed to this issue (Phillips & McQuarrie, 2009;

Halkias & Kokkinaki, 2011a).

The findings reveal an inverted-U pattern across the degrees of ad – brand incongruity for ad processing and evaluation. In more detail, ad – brand incongruity is found to stimulate ad processing, yet to a point that discrepancies can be successfully decoded and people can make sense of the communication. There seems to be a threshold in the magnitude of incongruity above which messages are ignored being perceived as irrelevant and non-diagnostic. Findings show that even though it becomes more difficult to make sense of the incoming message as incongruity increases, consumers do manage to accommodate moderate incongruities into existing brand knowledge and come up with a satisfactory interpretation of the ad’s concept. In contrast, ads that are extremely incongruent with the brand schema appear to significantly reduce consumers’

understanding of the communication. In addition, the favorable effect of schema incongruity is mediated by the participants’ ability to resolve the ad – brand discrepancy and produce a meaningful interpretation of the content of communication. As McQuarrie & Mick (2009) argue (see also Phillips & McQuarrie, 2009) enhanced pleasure may come from successfully decoding a more sophisticated and complex arrangement of signs. Accommodating incongruity can be thought of as solving a mental puzzle in which brand information and cues are combined in some sort of meaningful pattern to reveal the answer (Halkias & Kokkinaki, 2011a). The present research shows that when discrepancy is moderate this process is successful and generates a sense of satisfaction and fulfilment that is intuitively transferred to evaluations.

From a managerial perspective, incongruity-based tactics can enhance communication programmes by captivating consumers’ attention and making them to more actively participate in the communication process. However, great caution is required, since unresolved discrepancies may lead to frustration and disregard. Brand information that cannot be meaningfully reconciled with existing brand knowledge bears the risk of disorientating consumers and diluting the image of the brand. The positive effect of incongruent communication is mediated by the ability to successfully reconcile the ad – brand incongruity, hence marketing managers should ensure that consumers will have the ability and the opportunity to process a message of this type. Finally, the findings imply that such advertising tactics appear to be more appropriate for established and mature products to “refresh” and “rejuvenate” the brand.

References

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.

Berlyne, D. (1971). Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York: Appleton.

Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social Cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Goodstein, R. C. (1993). Category-Based Applications and Extensions in Advertising:

Motivating More Extensive Ad Processing, Journal of Consumer Research, 20(1), 87–99.

Halkias, G., & Kokkinaki, F. (2011a). Increasing Advertising Effectiveness through Incongruity- Based Tactics: The Moderating Role of Consumer Involvement, Journal of Marketing Communications. doi:10.1080/13527266.2011.592346 (accessed September 10, 2011).

Halkias, G., & Kokkinaki, F. (2011b). How Schema Incongruity Influences Consumer Responses: Exploring the Degree of Incongruity for Different Sources of Discrepancy.

European Advances in Consumer Research, 9, 144-150.

Houston, M. J., Childers, T. L., & Heckler, S. E. (1987). Picture-Word Consistency and the Elaborative Processing of Advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 24(4), 359-369.

Jhang, J. H., Grant, S. J., & Campbell, M. C. (2012). Get It? Got It. Good! Enhancing New Product Acceptance by Facilitating Resolution of Extreme Incongruity. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(2), 247-259.

Keller, K. L. (1993). Conceptualizing, Measuring, and Managing Customer-Based Brand Equity.

Journal of Marketing, 57(1), 1-22.

MacInnis, D. J., Moorman, C., & Jaworski, B. J. (1991). Enhancing and Measuring Consumers' Motivation, Opportunity, and Ability to Process Brand Information from Ads. Journal of Marketing, 55(4), 32-53.

Mandler, G. (1982). The Structure of Value: Accounting for Taste. In M. S. Clark & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and Cognition: The Seventeenth Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 3–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Maoz, E., & Tybout, A. M. (2002). The Moderating Role of Involvement and Differentiation in the Evaluation of Brand Extensions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 119-131.

McQuarrie, E. F., & Mick, D. G. (2009). A laboratory study of the effect of verbal rhetoric versus repetition when consumers are not directed to process advertising. International Journal of Advertising, 28(2), 287-312.

Meyers-Levy, J., Louie, T. A., & Curren, M. T. (1994). How Does the Congruity of Brand Names Affect Evaluations of Brand Name Extensions? Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(1), 46-53.

Noseworthy, T. J., Finlay, K., & Islam, T. (2010). From a Commodity to an Experience: The Moderating Role of Thematic Positioning on Congruity-Based Product Judgment.

Psychology & Marketing, 27(5), 465–486.

Phillips, B. J., & McQuarrie, E. F. (2009). Impact of Advertising Metaphor on Consumer Belief:

Delineating the Contribution of Comparison Versus Deviation Factors. Journal of Advertising 38(1), 49 - 62

Pieters, R., Warlop, L., & Wedel, M. (2002). Breaking Through the Clutter: Benefits of Advertisement Originality and Familiarity for Brand Attention and Memory. Management Science, 48(6), 765-781.

Rossiter, J. R., Percy, L., & Donovan, R. J. (1991). A better advertising planning grid. Journal of Advertising Research, 31(5), 11-21.

Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological Methodology 1982 (pp. 290-312). Washington DC: American Sociological Association.

Törn, F., & Dahlén, M. (2008). Effects of Brand Incongruent Advertising in Competitive Settings. European Advances in Consumer Research, 8, 234-239.

Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197-206.

How do Elderly Consumers Use the Internet? A Qualitative Study of Motivations and Usage Patterns  

 

KRISZTINA KOLOS (CORVINUS UNIVERSITY OF BUDAPEST)  

 

Co-author(s): Zsófia Kenesei (Corvinus University of Budapest)

Access to this paper is restricted to registered delegates of the EMAC 2013 Regional Conference.