• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Heart-Brain Coherence: the relationship between IHM Self-Regulation Techniques and Heart Rate Variability (HRV) Biofeedback Training among Iranian EFL Learners

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Share "Heart-Brain Coherence: the relationship between IHM Self-Regulation Techniques and Heart Rate Variability (HRV) Biofeedback Training among Iranian EFL Learners"

Copied!
11
0
0

Texto

(1)

ISSN 0976-2612, Online ISSN 2278–599X, Vol-7, Special Issue-Number4-June, 2016, pp854-864

http://www.bipublication.com

Case Report

Heart-Brain Coherence: the relationship between IHM Self-Regulation

Techniques and Heart Rate Variability (HRV) Biofeedback Training among

Iranian EFL Learners

Fatemeh Moussavi1 and Parviz Birjandi2* 1

Ph.D. Candidate in Applied Linguistics, Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran

2Professor of Applied Linguistics, Science and Research Branch,

Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran

*Corresponding Author Email: pbirjandi@srbiau.ac.ir

ABSTRACT:

Drawing on psycho-physiological research emphasizing the importance of facilitating the emotion-cognition or heart-brain interaction through self-regulation techniques, the present study aimed at investigating the efficacy of heart rate variability (HRV) on heart-brain coherence through self-regulation techniques suggested by Institute of HeartMath. Sixty-three Iranian university students and high school students participated in this study. The TestEdge program was used to enable students to self-generate an optimal psycho-physiological state in order to increase HRV measures and heart-brain coherence. Running paired samples t-tests and Wilcoxon Signed Ranks tests, the results confirmed that the application of IHM self-regulation techniques significantly reduced the subjects’ low coherence ratio but increased the mid and high coherence ratio on the posttest than the pretest among subjects. Moreover, the subjects had a lower heart rate on the posttest than the pretest. These results indicated that utilizing IHM self-regulation techniques increased heart-brain coherence ratio.

Key words:Biofeedback; Heart-Brain Coherence; Heart-Rate Variability; Self-Regulation Techniques; TestEdge Program

1. INTRODUCTION

Historically, heart was referred to as the center of wisdom and emotionbydivine religions and some philosophers such as Aristotle and Avicenna. Furthermore, earlier studies (Cannon,1927; Lacey & Lacey, 1978) in addition to more recentMRI neuro-imagingfindings provide evidence that the heart transmits complex patterns of neurological, hormonal, and electromagnetic information to the brain(Ardell, 1994; Armour, 2007; Armour & Ardell, 2004; Armour & Kember, 2004; McCraty, 2003b) which form a major component of the physiological backdrop that ultimately determines our emotional experience (Appelhans, & Luecken,

2006; Armour, 2007; Bradley,

(2)

changes in our emotional states (McCraty,

Atkinson, Tiller, Rein,&

Watkins,1995).Therefore, the analysis of HRV is one of the most valuable tools in examining the interactions between heart and brain or heart-brain coherence.

2. Review of the related literature

2.1. Emotional factors in learning

Coleman (2008) defines Emotional Intelligenceas the ability to monitor and use emotional

information. Suggested byGould, Cameron,

Daniels, Woolley, and McEwen (1992), negative emotions cause reduction in cell proliferation in subgranular zone (SGZ) of the dentate gyrus in the hippocampus while antidepressant treatments increase it. In 2001 Shors and her collogues found some potential roles for the SGZ in cognitive development. Research in the new discipline of neurocardiology has confirmed that the heart acts as a sophisticated information encoding and processing center (Armour & Ardell, 2004) which is particularly sensitive to changes in emotional states (Tiller, McCraty, & Atkinson, 1996). Stressful situationsincrease HRV and decrease the function of corpus callosum and cerebral cortex (Wolpow, Johnson, Hertel, & Kincaid, 2011) which leads to heart-brain incoherence and deficiency in learning.

2.2. Theoretical basis of heart-brain interaction

From the social cognitive learning theory (Bandura, 1986), much of human learning occurs in a social environment; learners have an ability to influence their own self-beliefs which can affect their learning (Bandura, 2001). Learners are active participants in learning (Dornyei, 2005) and move from other regulation to self-regulationto control their learning (Lantolf, 2006). This is in accordance with research in psychophysiology and neurobiology which show that learning emotional regulation techniques significantly enhances attention, memory, comprehension, and reasoning (Bradley, et al., 2009; Schore, 1994). Many researchers (e.g. Erbe, 2007;Joseph, 2010;Stober, 2004; Laskey & Hetzel, 2010)

suggest different self-regulation strategies like positive thinking, relaxation, self-assessment,think aloud, and reciprocal teaching. Wolpow, et al. (2011) presents a six-category self-care plan (pp. 58-59)to prevent the negative consequences of bad feelings.Furthermore, it has been shown that heartplays a critical role in regulating the autonomic nervous system and affecting our emotional experience (Armour, 2007; Bradley, McCraty, Atkinson, Arguelles, & Rees, 2007; Bradley, et al. 2009; Bradley, et al., 2010; Childre, & Rozman, 2005; Kim, 2011). By biofeedback training, one learns to control heart rate or other physical or mental processes(McCraty, 2001). Based on research on psychophysiology of emotions and heart–brain coherence (Arguelles, McCraty, & Rees, 2003; McCraty, 2005; McCraty, Atkinson, Tomasino, & Bradley,2009; Tiller, et al., 1996), the Institute of HeartMath (IHM) has developed some tools and techniques focusing on heart intuition (perceive information outside our consciousness, Pearsall, 1999) and heart intelligence (respond to emotionally relevant information, McCraty & Rees, 2009) that teach students skills to self-regulate stress and other emotional learning impediments (Arguelles et al., 2003; Bradley et al., 2007; McCraty, 2005). Presenting electrophysiological evidence of heart intuition, McCraty et al. (2004) found we can intentionally experience certain positive feelings to advantageously affect our heart rhythm which is an important indicator of one’s capacity to adapt effectively to environmental demands. Dalton (2013) mentionsa variety of HRV applications such as regulating autonomic nervous system, synchronizing brain alpha rhythms with the heart, and cognitive development. Assessing HRV is a

useful measurement of psychophysiological

(3)

nervous system are out of sync with each other(Childre & Rozeman, 2005; McCraty, 2001, 2005, 2011; Reich, 2009).

2.3. Neurological basis of heart-brain interaction

Neurologists discovered that the “rhythmic beating patterns of the heart are transformed into neural impulses that directly affect the electrical activity of the higher brain centers” (Childre & Martin, 1999, p. 11). They discovered a neural pathway whereby input from the heart to the brain could either inhibit or facilitate the brain’s electrical activity.

This interrelationship is explained based on field theory and holographic principles by Laszlo (1995, p. 100) and also by Kafatos and Nadeau (1990, p. 185) applying systems theory. Based on

holographic principles, intuitive perception

accesses a field of energy into which information about future events is spectrally enfolded. One method of measuring the neurological heart-brain

interaction is analyzing HRV from

electrocardiograms. The normal HRV is due to the synergistic action of the two autonomic nervous system branches. Comprised of the TestEdge program and emWave technology, the IHM goal is training to monitor heart rhythmsin order to achieve low levels of stress and better performance. Focusing on positive emotions is the first intervention to increase heart-brain coherence by self-regulation techniques such as Cut-Thru (Childre & Rozman, 2002),Freeze-Frame, Heart Lock-In (McCraty & Childre, 2003), and Neutral Tool (Childre & Rozman, 2005). A software program, emWave Desktop,collects and translates HRV data into graphicsto show if physical, cognitive, and emotional systems are in sync or not.

3. The study

The primary goal of the present study was toinvestigate the relationship between IHM

self-regulation techniques and heart-brain

coherence.The following question was addressed:

1. Is there any significant relationship

between students’ HRV and their use of IHM self-regulation techniques?

4. METHODOLOGY

4.1. Participants

The participants were chosen from seven intact classes among 43 Iranian university students (male and female) and 20 female high school students.

4.2. Instruments

The following instruments were used:1) The Student Activity Guide (Novosel, 2012) to present the understanding of treatment, 2) an adapted form of the Self-Care Checklist (Wolpow, et al., 2011)to deal with anxiety, 3)IHM self-regulation techniques to facilitate managing emotions and pay attention to the area of the heart with an intentional positive emotion in order to change afferent cardiac input pattern sent to the brain,and4) an emWave Desktop device to record the participants’ HRV.

4.3. Procedure

(4)

Rozman, 2002). All was carried out via interactive modeling of the techniques.

To measure HRV, a pulse sensor plugged into USB of a computer was clipped to the earlobe of each student to collect and translate HRV patterns into graphics. The more coherence was achieved, the smoother and more wavelike heart rhythm patterns became. Furthermore, a tri-colored bar graph indicated the degree of attained coherence: low,medium, andhigh coherence.

To determine participants’ heart-brain coherence, coherence level ratio and average heart rate of participants were measured before and after treatment.

4.4. Materials

Reading materials were selected from passages of Mosaic1 (Wehmann & Knezevic, 2008). In order to have same grade reading difficulty of pre- and posttest passages, Fry Graph Readability Formula (1968) was used to provide the measure of the readability.

4.5. Data gathering

To depict electrophysiological coherence scores, the participants were seatedabout four to five feet apart from the researcher in order to minimize the possibility of HRV interference from one heart to another. The data were stored in a data-storage of emWave Desktop.

4.6. Design

The design of the present study was one comparisongroup pre/posttest design (Best & Kahn, 2006). The between-subject factors were coherence level ratio and average heart rate.

Self-regulation techniques were considered as

independent variables and HRVwas considered as dependent variable.

5. Data analysis and results

The data were analyzed using both parametric (paired-samples t-test) and non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test).The first research question was decomposed into four minor questions because the heart rate variable was measured on a nominal scale while the coherence level ratios were percentages.

Moreover, the high, mid and low coherence level ratio could not be compared using a single analysis due to the fact that their total sum was 100.The normality of the data was checked. As displayed in Table 1, the ratios of skewness and kurtosis over their respective standard errors were all within the ranges of +/- 1.96 except for the pretest of high coherence ratio.

That is why the third minor null-hypothesis was analyzed using non-parametric Wilcoxon test.

Table 1. Testing Normality Assumption

N Skewness Kurtosis

Statistic Statistic Std. Error Ratio Statistic Std. Error Ratio

PreLowCoh 63 -.402 .302 -1.33 .352 .595 0.59

PreMidCoh 63 -.403 .302 -1.33 .080 .595 0.13

PreHighCoh 63 1.663 .302 5.51 4.043 .595 6.79

PostLowCoh 63 -.156 .302 -0.52 -.862 .595 -1.45

PostMidCoh 63 .486 .302 1.61 .874 .595 1.47

PostHighCoh 63 .460 .302 1.52 -.516 .595 -0.87

Note. Pre = pretest, Post = posttest and Coh = coherence ratio

5.1.1 Minor null-hypothesis 1-1

A paired samples t-test was run to compare the subjects’ low coherence ratio during the pretest and posttest. According to the results displayed in Table 2, the subjects had a lower low coherence ratio (M posttest =

(5)

Table 2. Paired Samples Statistics; Pretest and Posttest of Low Coherence Ratio

Mean N Std.

Deviation

Std. Error Mean Low Coherence

Level Ratio

Pretest 82.71 63 7.391 .931

Posttest 66.40 63 12.130 1.528

Figure 1: Means on Pretest and Posttest of Low Coherence Ratio

The results of paired-samples t-test (t (62) = 8.75, p = .000, r = .742 representing a large effect size) indicated that thelow coherence ratio level on the posttest was significantly lower. Thus the minor null-hypothesis 1-1 was rejected. The application of IHM self-regulation techniques reduced the subjects’ low coherence ratio significantly.

Table 3. Paired Samples Statistics; Pretest and Posttest of Low Coherence Ratio

Paired Differences

T Df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error Mean

95% Confidence Interval of the Difference

Lower Upper

16.317 14.795 1.864 12.591 20.044 8.754 62 .000

5.1.2 Minor null-hypothesis 1-2

To compare the subjects’ mid coherence ratio during the pretest and posttest, a paired samples t-test was run. Based on the results displayed in Table 4, after receiving IHM self-regulation techniques, they had a higher mid coherence ratio (M posttest = 16.48, SD = 5.66) compared with pretest (M pretest = 11.90, SD =

4.42).

Table 4. Paired Samples Statistics; Pretest and Posttest of Mid Coherence Ratio

Mean N Std.

Deviation

Std. Error Mean

Mid Coherence Level Ratio

Pretest 11.90 63 4.420 .557

Posttest 16.48 63 5.665 .714

(6)

Table 5. Paired Samples Statistics; Pretest and Posttest of Mid Coherence Ratio Paired Differences

T Df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error Mean

95% Confidence Interval of the Difference

Lower Upper

-4.571 7.321 .922 -6.415 -2.728 -4.956 62 .000

Figure 2: Means on Pretest and Posttest of Mid Coherence Ratio

5.1.3 Minor null-hypothesis 1-3

Based on the resultsof Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test runon the subjests’ high coherence ratio (Table 6), the difference between pre and posttest had a higher positive mean rank (MR = 32.34). In other words, the subjects showed a higher high coherence ratio on the posttest than the pretest.

Table 6. Mean and Sum of Ranks; Pretest and Posttest of High Coherence Ratio

N Mean Rank Sum of Ranks

PostHighCoh – PreHighCoh

Negative Ranks 5a 16.00 80.00

Positive Ranks 56b 32.34 1811.00

Ties 2c

Total 63

a. PostHighCoh < PreHighCoh b. PostHighCoh > PreHighCoh c. PostHighCoh = PreHighCoh

Based on the results of the Wilcoxon test (Z= -6.21, p=.000, r= .858 indicating a large effect size), there was a significant difference between pre and posttest of high coherence ratio and subjects had a higher mean rank on posttest. Thus, the minor null-hypothesis 1-3 was rejected.

Table 7. Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test Statisticsa

PostHighCoh – PreHighCoh

Z -6.219b

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed) .000

a. Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test b. Based on negative ranks.

(7)

5.1.4 Minor null-hypothesis 1-4

A Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test was run to compare the subjects’ average heart rate during the pretest and posttest. Based on the results displayed in Table 8 and Table 9, the subjects had a higher average heart rate on the pretest, i.e. the difference between pretest and posttest had a higher positive mean rank (MR = 35.79). It means there was a lower average heart rate of the subjects on the posttest than pretest.

Table 8. Mean and Sum of Ranks; Pretest and Posttest of Heart Ratio

N Mean Rank Sum of Ranks

PreAvHR – PostAvHR

Negative Ranks 13a 13.31 173.00

Positive Ranks 48b 35.79 1718.00

Ties 2c

Total 63

a. PreAvHR < PostAvHR b. PreAvHR > PostAvHR c. PreAvHR = PostAvHR

The results of the Wilcoxon test (Z = -5.55, p = .000, r = .766 representing a large effect size) and a lower mean rank on posttest indicated that there was a significant difference between the subjects’ heart rate on pretest and posttest. Thus the minor null-hypothesis 1-4 was rejected.

Table 9. Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test Statisticsa

PostHighCoh – PreHighCoh

Z -5.551b

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed) .000

a. Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test b. Based on negative ranks

Figure 4: Mean Ranks; Pretest and Posttest Average Heart Rate

6. DISCUSSION

Regarding the research question which

decomposed into four minor research questions, the results of paired-samples t-tests and Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test indicated that the application of IHM self-regulation techniques reduced the subjects’ low coherence ratio significantly but it increased the mid and high coherence ratio

significantly among subjects on average.

Moreover, there was a significant difference between the subject’s heart rate on pretest and

posttest. They had a lower mean rank on posttest. As it mentioned before, heart rhythm coherence biofeedback has been used in various educational settings with excellent results. For instance, McCraty et al. (1999) found that at-risk middle school children who were taught emotional

self-management skills exhibited significant

(8)

HRV is a kind of parasympathetic control over the heart and “more parasympathetic input results in more variable intervals between heart beats, that is, higher HRV” (p. 275). They conclude that HRV measures could be a likely candidate to index self-regulatory capacity and activity because brain structures involved in self-regulation and those involved in autonomic nervous system regulation overlap considerably, particularly with regard to the prefrontal cortex. The results of their study confirmed the case; subjects making a higher level of self-regulatory effort had higher HRV measures than subjects with lower self-regulatory effort (Segerstrom & Nes, 2007). In a study involving 60 sixth through eighth grade students, Arguelles et al. (2003) found increased stress resiliency in the training group who had

practiced IHM regulation techniques in

comparison to the control group that did not learn the technique (p. 19). Findings have been shown to correlate directly with improvement in cognitive performance, increased attention and focus, improved emotional competence and classroom behaviors, and reduced perception of stress (McCraty et al., 2009; Tiller, McCraty, & Atkinson, 1996).

Moreover, according to Fabes and Eisenberg (1997, cited in Appelhans & Leucken, 2006), higher levels of resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia have been associated with greater self-reported emotion regulation and the use of constructive coping strategies in university students. They found that those with lower resting respiratory sinus arrhythmia experienced greater negative emotional arousal in response to stress, which interfered with their ability to implement adaptive regulative strategies. Other investigation, in a similar vein, revealed that HeartMath’s programs are effective in improving HRV and emotional stability (Arguelles et al., 2003; Bradley et al., 2007; Bradley et al., 2009; Bradley et al., 2010; Lloyd, Brett, & Wesnes, 2010; McCraty, 2003b, 2005, 2011; McCraty et al., 1999).The result of the present study is totally in line with the results of these studies.

7. CONCLUSION

The new understanding of the physiology of positive emotions and the key role played by the heart in the generation of emotional experience have exciting implications for higher-order thinking skills, learner readiness, decision making, and test-taking, as well as for social and emotional behavior. Accordingly, practical tools have been developed that enable students to self-regulate the physiological processes underlying effective learning and performance. The most obvious conclusion which can be drawn from the above mentioned studies and this study is that all have generally supported the premise that higher HRV measures reflect a greater capacity for regulated emotional responses.

REFERENCES

1. Appelhans, B. M., & Luecken, L. J. (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated

emotional responding. Review of general

psychology, 10(3), 229-240.

2. Ardell, J. L. (1994). Anatomy and function of

mammalian intrinsic cardiac neurons. In JA.

3. Armour, & JL. Ardell (Eds.),

Neurocardiology (pp. 95–114). New York:

OUP.

4. Arguelles, L., McCraty, R., & Rees, R. A. (2003). The heart in holistic education.

Encounter: Education for meaning and social

justice, 16(3), 13-21.

5. Armour, J.A. (2007).The little brain on the

heart. Cleveland clinic Journal of

Medicine(74) 1, 848-851.

6. Armour, J. A. & Ardell J. L. (2004). Basic

and clinicalneurocardiology (Eds). New

York: OUP.

7. Armour, J. A. & Kember, G. C. (2004).

Cardiac sensory neurons. In JA. Armour, & J.

L. Ardell (Eds.), Basic and clinical

neurocardiology (pp.79–117). Oxford: OUP.

8. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of

thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

(9)

9. Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory:

An agentic perspective. Annual Review of

Psychology, 52, 1–26.

10. Best, J. W., & kahn, J. V. (2006). Research in

education (10th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education,

Inc.

11. Bradley, R., McCraty, R., Atkinson, M.,

Arguelles, L., & Rees, R. (2007). Reducing test anxiety and improving test performance in America’s schools:Results from the test edge

national demonstration study. Boulder Creek,

CA: HeartMath ResearchCenter, Institute of HeartMath.

12. Bradley, R., Atkinson, M., Tomasino, D.,

Rees, R., & Galvin, P. (2009). Facilitating emotional self-regulation in preschool children: Efficacy of the early heart smarts program in promoting social, emotional and

cognitive development.

13. Boulder Creek, CA:HeartMath LLC.

14. Bradley, R., McCraty, R., Atkinson, M.,

Tomasino, D., Daugherty, A., & Arguelles, L.

(2010). Emotion self-regulation,

psychophysiological coherence, and test anxiety: Results from an experiment using

electrophysiological measures.

AppliedPsychophysiology &

Biofeedback,35(4), 261-283.

15. Cannon, W.B. (1927). The James-Lange theory

of emotions: A critical examination and an

alternative theory. The American Journal of

Psychology, 39, 106–124.

16. Childre, D. (1998). Freeze-Frame: A

scientifically proven technique for clear

decision making and improved health (2nd

ed.).

17. Boulder Creek, CA: PlanetaryPublications.

18. Childre, D., & Martin, H. (1999). The

HeartMath solution. San Francisco: Harper.

19. Childre, D., & Rozman, D. (2002).

Overcoming emotional chaos. San Diego, CA:

Jodere Group.

20. Childre, D., & Rozman, D. (2005).

Transforming stress: The heartmath solution

for relieving worry, fatigue, and tension.

Oakland, CA: New HarbingerPublications.

21. Coleman, A. (2008). A dictionary of

psychology (3rd ed.). Oxford: OUP.

22. Coleman, M., & Vaughn, S. (2000). Reading

interventions for students with

emotional/behavioral disorders. Behavioral

Disorders, 25, 93-105.

23. Dalton, M. S. (2013). Measures of heart rate variability and how they relate to age, gender,

emotional behavior, and academic

achievement in elementary school children in

Adventist and public schools. Unpublished

doctoral dissertation, La Sierra University, CA.

24. Dornyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the

language learner: Individual differences in

second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

25. Erbe, B. (2007). Reducing test anxiety while increasing learning. College Teaching, 55 (3), 96-97.

26. Fry, E. B. (1968). A readability formula that

saves time. Journal of Reading, 11, 513–516.

27. Immordino-Yang, M. H., & Damasio, A.

(2007). We feel, therefore we learn: The relevance of affective and social

neuroscience to education. Mind, Brain,and

Education, 1(1), 3-10.

28. Joseph, N. (2010). Metacognition needed:

Teaching middle and high school students to develop strategic learning skills. Preventing

School Failure, 54(2), 99-103.

29. Kafatos, M., & Nadeau, R. (1990). The

conscious universe part and whole in modern

physical theory. New York: Sprinter-Verlag.

30. Kim, S. (2011). Heart rate variability

biofeedback and executive functioning in individuals with chronic traumatic brain

injury. Unpublished doctoraldissertation,

Yeshiva University, NY.

31. Lacey, J. I., & Lacey, B. C. (1978). Two-way

communication between the heart and the brain:Significance of time within the cardiac

(10)

32. Lantolf, J. (2006). Sociocultural theory and

L2. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,

28, 67-109.

33. Laskey, M. L., & Hetzel, C. S. (2010). Self regualted learning, metacognition and soft

skills: The 21st century learner. Online

submission to ERIC. (ERICDocument

Reproduction Service

34. No. ED511589.)

35. Laszlo, E. (1995). The interconnected

universe: Conceptual foundations of

transdisciplinary unified theory. Singapore:

World Scientific.

36. Lloyd, A., Brett, D., & Wesnes, K. (2010). Coherence training in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Cognitive

functions and behavioral changes. Alternative

Therapies in Health and Medicine, 16 (4),

34-42.

37. McCraty, R. (2001). Science of the heart.

Boulder Creek, CA: Institute of HeartMath.

38. McCraty, R. (2003a). Heart-brain

neurodynamics: The making of emotions. In D.

39. Childre , R. McCraty, & B. C. Wilson (Eds.),

Emotional sovereignty. Amsterdam: Harwood

Academic Publishers.

40. McCraty, R. (2003b).The scientific role of the

heart in learning and performance. Retrieved

from www.macquarieinstitute.com/scientific-

role-heart.

41. McCraty, R. (2004). The energetic heart:

Bioelectromagnetic communication within and between people. In P. J. Rosch & M. S.

Markov (Eds.), Clinicalapplications of

bioelectromagneticmedicine (pp. 541-562).

New York: Marcel Dekker.

42. McCraty, R. (2005). Enhancing emotional,

social, and academic learning with heart

rhythm coherence feedback. Biofeedback,

33(4), 130-134.

43. McCraty, R. (2011). Coherence: Bridging

personal, social and global health. Act Nerv

Super Rediviva, 53(3), 85–102.

44. McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Tiller, W. A.,

Rein, G., & Watkins, A. D. (1995).The effects of emotions on short term heart rate variability

using power spectrum analysis. American

Journal of Cardiology, 76(14), 1089-1093.

45. McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Tomasino, D.,

Goelitz, J., & Mayrovitz, H. N. (1999). The impact of an emotional self-management skills course on psychosocial functioning and autonomic recovery to stress in middle school

children. IntegrativePhysiological and

Behavioral Science, 34(4), 246-268.

46. McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Tomasino, D., & Bradley, R. T. (2009). The coherent heart: Heart-brain interactions, psychophysiological coherence, and the emergence of system-wide order. IntegralReview, 5(2), 10-115.

47. McCraty, R., & Childre, D. (2003). The

appreciative heart: The psychophysiology of

positive emotions and optimal functioning.

Boulder Creek, CA: Institute ofHeartMath. 48. McCraty, R., & Rees, R. A. (2009). The

central role of the heart in generating and sustaining positive emotions. In C. R. Snyder

& S. J. Lopez (Eds.), The Oxfordhandbook of

positive psychology (pp. 527-534). Oxford,

UK: OUP.

49. Nelson, R. J., Benner, G. J., & Rogers-Adkinson, D. L. (2003). An investigation of the characteristics of k-12 students with co-morbid emotional disturbance and significant language deficits served in public school settings. Behavioral Disorders, 9 (1), 25-33.

50. Novosel, L. C. (2012). Emotions

self-regulation: A mixed-methods intervention study of socioemotional and reading outcomes of high school students with reading

difficulties. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,

University of Texas atAustin, Austin.

51. Pearsall, P. (1999). The heart's code: Tapping

the wisdom and power of our heart energy.

New York, NY: Random House: Broadway Books.

(11)

and mental and emotional self-management.

Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco,California. 53. Segerstrom, S. C., & Nes, L. S. (2007). Heart

rate variability reflects self-regulatory

strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychological

Science, 18(3), 275-281.

54. Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of

emotional development. Hillsdale, New

Jersey: LawrenceErlbaum Associates.

55. Stober, J. (2004). Dimensions of test anxiety: Relations to ways of coping with pre-exam anxiety and uncertainty. Anxiety, Stress and

Coping, 17(3), 213-226.

56. Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection:

Further elaboration of a model of

neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience &

BiobehavioralReviews, 33(2), 81-88.

57. Tiller, W. A., McCraty, R., & Atkinson, M.

(1996). Cardiac coherence: A new,

noninvasive measure of autonomic nervous

system order. AlternativeTherapies in Health

and Medicine, 2(1), 52-65.

58. Wehmann, B. & Knezevic, M. (2008).

Mosaic1. Berkshire: McGrawHill Education.

59. Wolpow, R., Johnson, M., Hertel, R., &

Kincaid, S. (2011). The heart of learning and

teaching: Compassion, resiliency, and

academic success (2nd ed.).Washington:

Imagem

Table 1. Testing Normality Assumption
Table 4. Paired Samples Statistics; Pretest and Posttest of Mid Coherence Ratio
Figure 2: Means on Pretest and Posttest of Mid Coherence Ratio  5.1.3 Minor null-hypothesis 1-3
Table 8. Mean and Sum of Ranks; Pretest and Posttest of Heart Ratio

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Ocorre assim a primeira evolução do BSC, passando este de um sistema de medição de desempenho para um instrumento de gestão estratégia, ao conseguir transpor os objetivos

Tal recusa se faz, ora por negligência ao assunto - detectável principalmente nos estudos sobre cultura popular (Côrtes, 2000; Katz, 1989; Monteiro, 2011; Rodrigues, 2005;

Com base em cartografia existente, iconografia e textos relativos à cidade de São Tomé foram construídas plantas de várias épocas históricas, do Traçado Urbano

The results show that the Federal Laws prohibi- ting work for minors under 16 years of age has no considerable influence on the weekly hours worked by children and adolescents in

Após o teste das hipóteses, foi possível responder à pergunta de partida: “De que forma poderão as Forças Armadas mitigar a perda remuneratória na Reforma dos militares,

Tendo em conta as consequências possíveis que acarretam um edifico com uma deficiente QAI na saúde dos seus ocupantes, foi enunciada a seguinte questão de partida que

CONCLUSÕES: Antropometricamente a amostra está ajustada aos dados recolhidos na literatura nomeadamente na %MG; os valores da potência máxima aeróbia e VO2MÁX