• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials: Re(con)volution in Brain-Computer Interfacing.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Share "Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials: Re(con)volution in Brain-Computer Interfacing."

Copied!
22
0
0

Texto

(1)

Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials: Re

(con)volution in Brain-Computer Interfacing

Jordy Thielen*, Philip van den Broek, Jason Farquhar, Peter Desain

Radboud University Nijmegen, Donders Center for Cognition, Nijmegen, Netherlands

*jordy.thielen@gmail.com

Abstract

Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs) allow users to control devices and communicate by using brain activity only. BCIs based on broad-band visual stimulation can outperform BCIs using other stimulation paradigms. Visual stimulation with pseudo-random bit-sequences evokes specific Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials (BBVEPs) that can be reliably used in BCI for high-speed communication in speller applications. In this study, we report a novel para-digm for a BBVEP-based BCI that utilizes a generative framework to predict responses to broad-band stimulation sequences. In this study we designed a BBVEP-based BCI using modulated Gold codes to mark cells in a visual speller BCI. We defined a linear generative model that decomposes full responses into overlapping single-flash responses. These sin-gle-flash responses are used to predict responses to novel stimulation sequences, which in turn serve as templates for classification. The linear generative model explains on average 50% and up to 66% of the variance of responses to both seen and unseen sequences. In an online experiment, 12 participants tested a 6 × 6 matrix speller BCI. On average, an online accuracy of 86% was reached with trial lengths of 3.21 seconds. This corresponds to an Information Transfer Rate of 48 bits per minute (approximately 9 symbols per minute). This study indicates the potential to model and predict responses to broad-band stimulation. These predicted responses are proven to be well-suited as templates for a BBVEP-based BCI, thereby enabling communication and control by brain activity only.

Introduction

A Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) enables one to control a device by using brain activity only, bypassing the peripheral nervous system. Because BCI is not dependent on muscle control, it provides an additional output channel for the brain to be used for communication and control.

The online BCI cycle can be initiated by specific stimulation that evokes characteristic task-related brain activity. During stimulation, brain activity is commonly measured by electroen-cephalogram (EEG) recordings. A computer then interprets the measured EEG following sev-eral pre-processing steps (e.g., cleaning the data) and machine learning techniques (e.g., learning task-related and subject-specific brain activity). With this analysis a computer can decode and detect task-related information from brain activity. The output is usually fed back OPEN ACCESS

Citation:Thielen J, van den Broek P, Farquhar J, Desain P (2015) Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials: Re(con)volution in Brain-Computer Interfacing. PLoS ONE 10(7): e0133797. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0133797

Editor:Daniele Marinazzo, Universiteit Gent, BELGIUM

Received:April 11, 2015

Accepted:July 1, 2015

Published:July 24, 2015

Copyright:© 2015 Thielen et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the

Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability Statement:Before conducting the experiments, an Ethical Approval and Informed Consent of participants were obtained. This restricts us from making data available at a public repository. Anonymized data routines are available upon request (jordy.thielen@gmail.com,info@donders.ru.nl). Processed data are available at DANS (http://dx.doi. org/10.17026/dans-zth-37cr). Matlab routines are available at GitHub (https://github.com/thijor/ Reconvolution).

(2)

to the user after which a new cycle starts. The cyclic diagram as shown inFig 1forms a general

framework against which most BCI research can be positioned [1].

In this study we focus on a visual matrix speller, first proposed by [2]. A typical visual

matrix speller presents a grid to the user, each cell containing one symbol. During stimulation, these cells alter in luminance (i.e., they flash). By visually attending a cell, a cell-specific Visu-ally Evoked Potential (VEP) is elicited, which is detectable by a BCI and enables the selection of individual symbols. At least five different stimulation paradigms can be used to produce such

recognizable brain patterns (for a review see [3]). Commonly, an oddball sequence is used that

evokes a distinct time-locked Event-Related Potential for targets (i.e., Time Domain Multiple

Access, e.g., see P300-based speller [4]), or periodic flicker that evokes distinct Steady-State

Visually Evoked Potentials (i.e., Frequency Domain Multiple Access, e.g., see SSVEP-based

speller [5]). Instead, in this study we focus on Code Division Multiple Access schemes for

Fig 1. The BCI cycle.A general framework of BCIs, showing the consecutive steps in signal processing. These steps involve stimulation in a specific modality, data measurement and pre-processing, data analysis, and output or feedback generated by the BCI. Adapted from [1].

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g001

BBVEPs: Re(con)volution in BCI

Education, Culture and Science (SSM06011, P. Desain).

(3)

stimulation. These schemes minimize the cross-stimuli interference by making the cell-specific stimulation patterns (i.e., the sequences of flashes) orthogonal to each other. This is particularly achieved by using pseudo-random noise-codes. In the visual domain these can be applied as flash sequences that evoke specific Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials (BBVEPs). The BBVEP-based speller, though uncommon in current literature, has outperformed spellers that

use more common stimulation schemes [6].

The BBVEP-based matrix speller was first proposed by [7] and tested with an ALS patient

using intracranial electrodes and an 8 × 8 grid. The patient could write up to 10–12 words per

minute [8]. More recently, the BBVEP-based matrix speller has been adapted to increasingly

successful use with EEG recordings. [6] used one 63-bit m-sequence for stimulation.

Essen-tially, multiple responses to this bit-sequence were acquired and averaged to obtain a template. Both bit-sequence and template are circularly shifted in time to create stimuli and templates for multiple classes. New data is classified by selecting the best matching template by means of

highest correlation. With a 4 × 4 grid and pre-selected EEG channels, [6] reached a

communi-cation rate of 92.8 bits per minute. By deriving optimal spatial filters with Canonical Correla-tion Analysis (CCA) and a 4 × 8 grid, this communicaCorrela-tion rate was improved to 108 bits per

min [9]. Instead of a simple averaging approach to create templates, a Once Class Support

Vec-tor Machine (OCSVM) can better estimate the probability distribution of high-dimensional data. Applying a OCSVM together with CCA resulted in a communication rate of 133.6 bits

per minute [10]. This communication rate was improved to 144 bits per minute—the highest

speed reported so far—by implementing an adaptive classifier [11].

In this study, we investigate a novel approach to synchronous BBVEP-based BCI. First, con-trary to an m-sequence, Gold codes are used. Gold codes are pseudo-random bit-sequences

generated by combining two carefully selected m-sequences [12]. A set of Gold codes contains

numerous bit-sequences that have an optimized (i.e., minimized) cross-correlation. Thus, Gold codes can be directly applied to classification problems with a higher number of classes. Addi-tionally, a Gold code has an optimized auto-correlation enabling their use in an asynchronous BBVEP-based BCI (i.e., continuous stimulation without any phase-lock). A set of m-sequences has no guaranteed good cross-correlation. However, any m-sequence does have a good auto-correlation which makes it possible to create a set of circular-shifts of the same m-sequence

(e.g., [6,10,11]). A set of circular-shifted m-sequences thus exhibits good cross-correlation,

but the possibility for asynchronous use is lost.

Second, in this study the EEG responses to these Gold codes are modeled with a generative model. This generative model, referred to as reconvolution, can be trained quickly in order to

generate template EEG responses to bit-sequences [13]. To put reconvolution to the test, two

sets of Gold codes are used. The first set is used for stimulation during the training phase of the BCI. Recorded responses to this first set are used to train reconvolution, in order to predict responses to the second set. This second set is used for stimulation during the testing phase of the BCI. Thus, a clear separation exists between training and testing, which will only succeed if reconvolution can truly generate and predict responses well.

Third, with reconvolution EEG responses to any bit-sequence can be predicted, either seen (i.e., used during training) or unseen. Though, potentially only a subset of stimulation sequences is required. Therefore an optimization pipeline is possible: a subset of codes is selected, allocated to the speller grid according to the dissimilarity of their predicted responses.

Additionally, an early stopping algorithm is defined [14]. To summarize, the contribution of

(4)

Methods

Participants

Twelve university students (4 male, mean age 24.0 years, standard deviation 2.3 years) partici-pated in the experiment after pre-screening and providing written informed consent. Pre-screening consisted of excluding participants with any history of epilepsy. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no central nervous system abnormalities. After the experiment participants were paid for their contribution to this study. The experi-ment was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Faculty of Social Sciences at the Radboud University Nijmegen.

Equipment

The speller was presented on a 24 inch BenQ XL2420T LED monitor with 120 Hertz refresh rate and a resolution of 1920 × 1080 pixels. Timing accuracy was measured with a light-sensor and was always within 2 ms. The light intensity of white, black and grey were 185 lux, 4 lux and 55 lux, respectively.

Participants were seated in a chair at a distance of 75 cm from the monitor. The height of the chair was adjusted so that the participant was facing the center of the monitor.

EEG data was recorded with 64 sintered Ag/AgCl active electrodes according to the 10–20

system, amplified by a Biosemi ActiveTwo amplifier. Data was acquired at 2048 Hertz, though the data was immediately down-sampled to 360 Hertz to match a multiple of the stimulation frequency (i.e., the 120 Hertz monitor refresh rate). Data was pre-processed following linear

de-trending, common average referencing, and spectral filtering with two pass-bands at 5−48

Hertz and 52−100 Hertz. Processed data is available at DANS (http://dx.doi.org/10.17026/

dans-zth-37cr, [15]). The raw EEG data is stored at the Donders Center for Cognition, and can

be requested by e-mail (info@donders.ru.nl).

Stimuli

The cells in the 6 × 6 speller grid were presented either black or white. The flash sequences could thus be represented as binary sequences where ones and zeros represented white and

black cells respectively. In this study two sets of Gold codes were used [12]. The first set,

denotedV, was used for training and is generated with a linear feedback shift register of

lengthm= 6 and feedback taps at positions 6,5,2,1 and at 6,1 (for the generation process see

[12,16]). The second set, denotedU, was used for testing and was generated with the same

register, but with feedback taps at positions 6,5,3,2 and at 6,5. BothVandUwere modulated

by xor-ing them with a double-frequency bit-clock, which limited low-frequency content.V

andUcontainedn= 2m+ 1 = 65 bit-sequences that all had a length ofl= 2(2m

−1) = 126

bits. One such bit-sequence had a duration oftb= 126/120 = 1.05 s. These modulated Gold

codes were sequences of short on-off runs (i.e.,‘10’or‘100’) and long on-off runs (i.e.,‘110’

or‘1100’), representing short and long flashes, respectively. These two flashes were of length

ts= 1/1201000 = 8.3 ms andtl= 2/1201000 = 16.6 ms, respectively. For an illustration of

these modulated Gold codes, seeFig 2.

Calibration

A template matching classifier was constructed to identify the attended cell. In template match-ing, the best fitting template is chosen using a similarity measure. In this study we used correla-tion to measure the similarity between a single-trial and several templates, which is maximized

(5)

to obtain a class-label:

y¼ arg max

i

x>T i

ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi

x>xT>T

p ð1Þ

whereyis the predicted class label,xis the single-trial andTiis the template of classi.

The classifier, including the templates, was constructed following five consecutive steps: template generation, spatial filtering, subset optimization, layout optimization, and learning of

stopping criteria (seeFig 3). Matlab routines are available at GitHub (https://github.com/

thijor/Reconvolution).

Template generation: Reconvolution. To be able to identify the attended cell using

tem-plate matching, temtem-platesTiare required. In order to obtain these,ktrials were recorded while

bit-sequences fromVwere presented. Normally, averaging trials of one class would yield a

tem-plate. However, this would require many trials to cover all bit-sequences (i.e., say 100 trials for each of 65 bit-sequences), which is infeasible. Instead, we defined a generative method for tem-plate generation: reconvolution.

We exploit the fact that the modulated Gold codes are built as a sequence of only two events: short and long flashes. Assuming linearity of the brain response, the response to a sequence of flashes can be found by convolution: adding time shifted versions of single-flash responses. Conversely, these single-flash responses can be found by deconvolution, which boils down to linear regression. Brain processes behave non-linearly as well, though assuming linearity has

proven to be sufficient for modeling of specific early visual responses (e.g., see [17]).

Fig 2. Stimulation sequences.Four modulated Gold codes are shown ofl= 2*(2m

−1) = 126 bits. These are pseudo-random binary-codes that are sequences of short on-off runs (i.e.,‘10’or‘100’) and long on-off runs (i.e.,‘110’or‘1100’). These on-off runs are used to modulate the luminance of the cells, and thus represent short and long flashes.

(6)

Reconvolution is a two step approach for template generation combining deconvolution and

convolution (seeFig 4). First, responses to individual flashes are estimated by decomposing

full-responses (i.e., the estimation-step). Second, full-responses to (un)seen bit-sequences are generated by applying the estimated single-flash responses (i.e., the generation-step).

The first step in reconvolution is estimation. Acquired data is decomposed into a linear

combination of a structure matrix and responses. In [18], a study on auditory noise-tagging,

responses to rising (‘01’) and falling (‘10’) events in the bit-sequence were estimated. In this

study, the decomposition is based on short (i.e.,‘10’or‘100’) and long flashes (i.e.,‘110’or

‘1100’). We call the responses to these single-flashes pulse responses, not to be confused with

impulse responses (to Dirac impulses). The generic model of decomposition for a single-chan-nel single-trial can be written as:

~

xðtÞ ¼X L

t¼1

IsðtÞrsðt tÞ þIlðtÞrlðt tÞ ð2Þ

Fig 3. The online pipeline.Three stages exist: training, calibration and testing. During training, responsesXto stimuli fromVare recorded. During calibration,Xis deconvolved to pulse responsesrusingV. Template responsesTVandTUare generated by convolving theserwith the bit-sequencesVand

U, respectively. Templates are multiplied (circles) with filters (WX,WT) designed by CCA. The subset and layout are optimized givingU0andTU0, and stopping

marginsmare learned. In the testing phase, a new single-trialxis assigned the class-labelythat maximizes the correlation between the spatiallyfiltered single-trialxand templatesT0

U. The classifier emits the class-label if the maximum correlation exceeds the threshold margin. In the case wherein the margin is

not reached, more data is collected.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g003

(7)

wherexðtÞ~ is a modeled full-response at timet,I

s(t) andIl(t) are indicator functions that have

the value 1 if there is a short/longflash at timetand 0 elsewhere,rs(t) andrl(t) are the pulse

responses to short/longflashes at timet, andLis the length of bothrsandrl. This relationship

can be expressed in a more compact matrix notation:

~

x¼MsrsþMlrl¼ ½Ms MlŠ rs

rl

2

4 3

5 ¼Mr ð3Þ

Fig 4. Reconvolution.Reconvolution is a two-step approach: estimation (top) and generation (bottom). In the first step a recorded responsex(t) is decomposed according to the structure in a bit-sequencesv(t). The decomposition yields transient responsesrsandrlto short and long pulses, respectively.

In the second step, these estimated responses can be used to generate or predict the responseT(t) to a (unseen) bit-sequencesu(t).

(8)

Here,~xis a column vector ofssamples.M

sandMlare structure matrices that have the value 1

in column 1 at rowtif there is a short/longflash at timet. For columns 2 toLones shift down

tot+L.MsandMlare 0 in all other locations.MsandMlare both of sizes×Land thusMis of

sizes× (2L). An example of a structure matrix is illustrated inFig 5. Bothrsandrlare column

vectors ofLsamples and thusris of size (2L) × 1. This model can be generalized to multiple

single-channel single-trials by concatenation, to form the following linear regression problem:

X¼½x1 xkŠ ¼½M1 MkŠr¼Mr ð4Þ

whereXis the concatenation ofksingle-channel single-trials ((sk) × 1), andMis the

concate-nation of all correspondingM((sk) × (2L)). This means thatrcan be found solving the linear

Fig 5. Structure Matrix.In reconvolution a structure matrixMis used that indicates when a certain pulse happens in a bit-sequences(t).Mis the concatenation ofMsandMl, both having the value 1 whenever a

short/long pulse happens at timet, shifting downLrows. This matrix is used to (de)compose a full-responsex

(t) to pulse responsesrsandrlto short/long flashes.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g005

(9)

equation; the solution can be found as follows:

r¼MþX ð5Þ

whereM+is the pseudo-inverse ofM.

The second step in reconvolution is generation. Recall that theksingle-trials used

stimula-tion sequences fromV, hence the full-responses to bit-sequences inVcan be generated,

denoted as templatesTV. Sinceris estimated, any bit-sequence that is built up from the same

events (here short and long flashes) can be predicted by constructing its structure matrixM

and multiplying it with the response vectorr. Thus, all responses to bit-sequences inUcan be

predicted as well, denoted as templatesTU:

TV¼MVr ð6Þ

TU ¼MUr ð7Þ

When the estimation and generation steps are repeated for each channel, reconvolution

generatesTVandTUcontaining the (predicted) templates forVandUrespectively. BothTV

andTUhave dimensionsc×s×n(i.e., channels by samples by templates).

Spatial filtering: Canonical Correlation Analysis. EEG is a noisy signal. To increase the signal to noise ratio (SNR), a noise reduction method such as spatial filtering is often applied. A spatial filter projects the signals from sensor space into source space, where sources are sepa-rated by any criterium. For each component, the filter combines electrode signals into a weighted sum. This multivariate approach usually outperforms feature selection by channel

picking (e.g., [6,10]). Here we use Canonical Correlation Analysis (CCA). In short, this works

by estimating a spatial filter, which spatially (i.e., across EEG channels) averages (i.e., a

weighted average) each single-trial time-point to estimate a component’s signal strength. This

component’s signal strength is as close as possible to the signal strength estimated for the same

time-point when temporally averaging all trials. CCA does this by deriving two spatial filters

WXandWT, in order to maximize the correlation betweenW>XXandWT>T:

WX;WT ¼ arg max

WX;WT

W> XXT>WT

ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi

W>

XXX>WXWT>TT>WT

p ð8Þ

Here,Xrepresents the concatenated single-trials of sizec× (sk).Trepresents a matrix of

simi-lar dimensionality, though it contains the corresponding generated templatesTV. CCA derives

multiple uncorrelated components for bothWXandWT, sorted on canonical correlation.

Because we want to optimize the correlation between single-trials and corresponding

tem-plates, thefirst component suffices. The templates are spatiallyfiltered withWTleavingTVand

TUto be of sizes×n. The data is spatiallyfiltered withWXleavingXof sizes×k.

Subset optimization: Platinum subset. Ucontainsn= 65 codes and for each code, the

responsesTUare predicted. In designing a BCI user interface sometimes onlypcodes are

needed, wherepn. Note that while the stimuli are all uncorrelated as binary signals, this

does not imply that the responses are uncorrelated too. Therefore a selection process aims at

finding an easily response-distinguished subset of codes. BecauseTUis known, it is possible to

calculate the full cross-correlation matrix of responses. However, choosing a subset ofpcodes

from a set ofncodes by exhaustive search is infeasible, unlesspis close to 1 orn. The

combina-torial explosion is harnessed in two steps: clustering and selection (seeFig 6).

First, responses are hierarchically clustered with single linkage to minimize within-group

correlation and maximize between-group correlation [19]. Then,kclusters are obtained by

(10)

Fig 6. Platinum subsets.The steps for finding a subset ofp= 3 codes from a set ofn= 10 codes is shown. First the full set is clustered grouping similar points (A1till A3). Then, iteratively (B till D) each cluster is collapsed into a single point by selecting one candidate. This candidate is chosen by maximizing the distance to all other living points outside the cluster (B2, C2, D2). The remaining points form the Platinum subset (D3).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g006

(11)

Second, the best cluster-defining codes are selected in a greedy way. The code from within a cluster that minimizes the maximum response-correlation with all codes outside the cluster, is chosen as the best candidate for a cluster. The cluster is then cropped to this one candidate. This selection process starts with a random cluster and is iterated over all clusters until all

con-tain only one code. These remaining codes form the Platinum subsetU02Ucontainingp

codes. In addition,T0

U 2TUcontains onlyptemplates. In this study,p= 36 as we use a 6 × 6

matrix for the speller application.

Layout optimization. The stimuli are arranged in a 6 × 6 matrix of cells. Depending on the visual angle, codes used for neighbouring cells may leak through in the responses. An opti-mal layout allocates codes to cells in such a way that neighbouring cells do not correlate much in their responses. For a 6 × 6 speller matrix, given the Platinum subset of 36 codes, an optimal layout cannot be selected by exhaustive enumeration. Instead, a greedy optimization is used.

The algorithm starts with an arbitrary allocation of codes to matrix cells. Then, the worst neighbouring code-pair is selected by means of largest pair-wise correlation in neighbouring

templates fromTU. The algorithm searches exhaustively for the best exchange of two codes

that gives the least pair-wise correlation between any vertical, horizontal, or diagonal bours. The algorithm performs the exchange and continues with finding the next worst neigh-bour until the exchange does not result in a better layout. Because global optimality is not guaranteed, the algorithm is restarted with several random initial layouts. From these opti-mized layouts, the layout with the lowest maximal pair-wise correlation in neighbouring tem-plates is selected. The improvement of such a layout compared with an initial one is depicted inFig 7. BothU0andT0

Uare ordered according to this optimized layout.

Learning of stopping criteria. When enough training data is available, one can determine a fixed time interval needed for a desired classifier performance. On the contrary, one could also define a criterion to dynamically decide when trials can be stopped, called early-stopping

(for an overview see [20]). In online processing of new data, the length of a single-trial is

sequentially increased as long as the classifier produces a criterion value below a required mar-gin. If the classifier is sufficiently certain, the trial stops and the classifier presents its output. This approach provides a possible speed gain and an easy adaptation to occasional distractions.

Fig 7. Layout optimization.To prevent cross-talk between neighbouring cells the allocation of sequences to cells is optimized. An initial (left) and optimized (right) layout are shown. Numbers indicate bit-sequences. The shade indicates the correlation between responses to codes from neighbouring cells. The correlations are depicted between horizontal, vertical, and diagonal neighbours. For diagonal neighbours, the maximum correlation of the two diagonals is shown. In this perspective darker colours represent less correlation and better neighbours, hence an increased potential to distinguish.

(12)

Here, the margin—the correlation difference between best and second best matching

tem-plates—is used to indicate that a decision on the best matching template can be made reliably.

The algorithm learns these stopping marginsmbased on the evolving distributions of

correla-tion values condicorrela-tioned on the correctness of outcome. Moreover, the full cross-correlacorrela-tion

matrix of thektrials fromXwith the templates inTVis computed. Then, the difference

between the best and second best matching template is estimated and separated in two distri-butions: one containing correct trials (i.e., the best matching template was the correct one), and the other containing incorrect trials. Using these distributions a threshold margin is learned by lowering it until a desired performance is reached. This analysis is repeated for each possible trial length independently.

In our matrix speller the maximum trial length was 4.2 seconds. This trial length started with a segment of 100 milliseconds and increased with 100 milliseconds each iteration. Hence,

mcontains 42 learned stopping margins. These margins were fitted with an exponential

func-tion; the first five margins (i.e., the first 500 ms) were artificially set to 1 to enforce a minimal length of data; the last margin (i.e., at 4.2 s) was set to 0 to force a decision at the maximum trial length.

These marginsmwere also used to provide the user with feedback regarding the classifier’s

certainty. This was communicated by outlining the edges of all cells with a specific colour. At the start of a trial all edges were coloured gray. As the trial progresses, edges were coloured green whenever the cell-specific template reached the margin. Conversely, the cell-specific tem-plates that did not reach the margin were coloured red. The colouring was scaled to the mar-gins and the minimum and maximum correlation to better visualize which cells were most and least likely to be chosen. Because of the scaling, multiple cells could be coloured green, though

the highest correlating cell is the greenest.Fig 8shows four chronological snap-shots of a trial

in which the target was‘T’.

Experiment

Each participant performed the experiment in one session which was separated in four blocks. In chronological order, the experiment consisted of: a practice block, containing two runs; a training block, containing one run; a copy-spelling block, containing six runs; and a free-spell-ing block, containfree-spell-ing one run. In between the trainfree-spell-ing and copy-spellfree-spell-ing block, the template matching classifier was calibrated according to the full pipeline, as described above.

Fig 8. Colour feedback.While a trial progresses, colour feedback is given regarding the classifier’s certainty. All cells start gray (A). A cell is coloured more green if the cell is more likely to be selected, whereas a cell is coloured more red when it is likely to not be selected. The colours are scaled to the specific margin and the maximum and minimum correlation between the single-trial and templates. Here, the target was‘T’.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g008

(13)

In all runs–except in the free-spelling block–participants were asked to focus at each sym-bol in the 6 × 6 matrix once, therefore a run contains 36 trials. The order of symsym-bols was ran-domized, thus participants were performing a random copy-spelling task. A trial started by colouring the target-cell green for 1 second. After target instruction all symbols flashed accord-ing to their specific bit-sequence immediately for 4.2 seconds (i.e., four code repetitions). Directly after stimulation, the output of the classifier was presented by colouring the cell blue for 1 second. After this feedback, the next trial was initiated immediately.

In the practice block, the first thirty-six codes from code-setVwere used. Here, stimulation

was adjusted in such a way that the target-cell was always flashing with the same bit-sequence. This was achieved by switching a predefined practice code with the instructed code for each

trial. In this way, the participant always focused on the first code fromVin the first run and

the second code fromVin the second run. The practice block was used to familiarize the

par-ticipant with the task and the concept of a visual speller. In addition, via these two runs, multi-ple trials were acquired of two bit-sequences that were used for offline analysis, discussed later on. Because no classifier was yet defined, feedback was always at the instructed cell.

In the training block, also the first thirty-six codes fromVwere used. Thus, the participant

attended to each bit-sequence in this subset once. Because no classifier was yet defined,

feed-back was always as if correct. Thesek= 36 trials were used to calibrate the classifier.

In the copy-spelling block, the bit-sequences fromU0were used for stimulation. The

copy-spelling block was divided in two conditions: fixed-length and early-stopping. Both conditions contained three runs. In the fixed-length runs, stimulation was always 4.2 seconds long. In the early-stopping runs stimulation was stopped whenever the classifier could make a decision

based on the marginsm. The order of the six runs was randomized over participants, though

participants were always told which condition followed next.

The free-spelling block was similar to the copy-spelling block, though was always in early-stopping mode. During this run there were no targets instructed, but still 1 second was reserved for making up the next decision. Participants could maximally spell 50 symbols, after which

the speller stopped automatically. By selecting the‘_’symbol a space was written; the‘<’

func-tioned as a backspace; the‘#’had to be selected twice in a row to exit the speller; by selecting

either a‘!’,‘?’or‘.’twice in a row, text-to-speech software was used to pronounce the sentence,

after which the sentence was cleared.

Validation of BCI performance

Validation of the online performance of the BCI is estimated by the accuracy and speed on all random copy-spelling trials, and all free-spelling trials. The accuracy is defined as the percent-age of correctly classified trials. The speed is measured as the Information Transfer Rate (ITR)

[21]. In addition to the ITR, the Symbols Per Minute (SPM) measure is used to approximate a

more practical speed, as it incorporates the necessity to perform a backspace after an incorrect

selection [22]. The measures are as follows:

B¼ log2NþPlog2Pþ ð1 PÞlog2

1 P

N 1½bitsŠ ð9Þ

ITR¼ ðT=60Þ B½bits=minŠ ð10Þ

SPM¼ ðT=60Þ ðP ð1 PÞÞ½sym=minŠ ð11Þ

whereNis the number of classes,Pthe accuracy in a range of 0 to 1, andTthe number of

(14)

experiment, the inter-trial time was always 2 seconds (i.e., 1 second instruction and 1 second feedback).

Validation of reconvolution

The generative power of reconvolution is estimated by means of the explained variance. More specifically, the more variance in the data is explained by reconvolution, the better the template is predicted. Here we chose to compare the predicted templates from reconvolution with ERPs. The practice block data, acquired for this purpose, provides the necessary multiple trials for both modulated Gold codes to be able to construct ERPs.

In both runs in the practice block,k= 36 trials were collected. These sets are denotedX1for

code 1 andX2for code 2, both with dimensionsc×s×k. Here,scontains four code repetitions.

Both sets are sliced to single code repetitions, so bothX1andX2are of sizecs

4 ðk4Þ.

The validation of reconvolution is done usingk-fold cross-validation. BothX1andX2are

split intokfolds. Reconvolution is applied to bothX1andX2separately, using one fold. This

gives two templates matricesT1andT2, both of sizecs

42. The remainingk−1 folds are

used to construct the ERPs by averaging inX1andX2separately. This gives two ERPsR1and

R2of sizec4. Now the explained variance can be estimated by computing the squared corre-s

lation between the prediction and the ERP. This is done for seen sequences (i.e., self-prediction,

e.g.,T1predictsR1) and unseen sequences (i.e., cross-prediction, e.g.,T1predictsR2).

Valida-tion is performed for each electrode individually and for spatiallyfiltered responses using CCA

as defined before.

Validation of optimizations

Three optimizations were performed: subset optimization, layout optimization, and early-stopping.

To validate the effectiveness of subset optimization, the training block data is used to

con-struct templatesTifor all codes. A paired t-test is then performed between several random

sub-sets and the Platinum subset. Here, dependent variables are the maximum, mean, and minimum correlation within a subset.

To validate the effectiveness of layout optimization, the training block data is used to con-struct the Platinum subset. A paired t-test is then performed between several random layouts and the optimized layout. Here, dependent variable are the maximum, mean, and minimum correlation within a subset.

To validate the effectiveness of early-stopping, the ITRs as computed in the validation of the BCI performance are considered. A paired t-test is performed to test for any significant effect between the two conditions: fixed-length and early-stopping.

Results

Validation of BCI performance

In the copy-spelling block, the average classification rate in the fixed-length condition was 86 percent. This yields an ITR of 38.12 bits per minute and an SPM of 6.93 symbols per minute. In the early-stopping condition, the average classification rate remained 86 percent but on average data segments of 3.21 seconds were sufficient to make a valid decision. This yields an ITR of approximately 48.37 bits per minute and an SPM of 8.99 symbols per minute. Recall that early-stopping was always trained using a targeted classification rate of 95 percent, which was not feasible for all participants. Note that the speed goes up for almost all participants when early-stopping is used. In the calculation of the ITR and SPM an inter-trial interval of 2

(15)

seconds was always incorporated. The results on the random copy-spelling task are

summa-rized inTable 1.

During the free-spelling block, participants were able to express full sentences according to their intentions. Some participants (N = 9) were even able to spell a full sentence without mak-ing a smak-ingle mistake or by performmak-ing correctly selected backspaces. Amongst the participants

full sentences like“great experiment on Friday thank you”were easily achieved. Results of the

free-spelling task are listed inTable 2.

Table 1. Copy-spelling performance.Performance rates of all individual participants and the grand average during random copy-spelling. Accuracies (P) are given in percentages of correct classifications together with the average trial time needed for classification (including overhead: inter-trial time). The Infor-mation Transfer Rate (ITR) and Symbols Per Minute (SPM) are estimated (including the inter-trial time). Note that the early-stopping pipeline was trained with a fixed targeted accuracy of 95 percent for all participants.

Fixed-Length Early-Stopping

P T ITR SPM P T ITR SPM

% sec bits/min sym/min % sec bits/min sym/min

S01 83 6.20 35.47 6.45 82 5.41 39.89 7.19

S02 100 6.20 50.03 9.68 97 3.44 84.41 16.46

S03 93 6.20 42.67 8.24 91 5.61 45.43 8.71

S04 98 6.20 47.82 9.32 99 3.94 76.86 14.95

S05 94 6.20 43.46 8.42 94 4.14 65.02 12.60

S06 58 6.20 19.87 1.61 76 6.16 30.57 5.05

S07 65 6.20 23.51 2.87 68 6.14 25.41 3.44

S08 92 6.20 41.89 8.06 87 5.26 45.05 8.45

S09 93 6.20 42.67 8.24 85 6.14 37.18 6.88

S10 83 6.20 35.47 6.45 81 5.94 34.96 6.17

S11 90 6.20 40.38 7.71 94 4.64 59.17 11.49

S12 81 6.20 34.15 6.09 81 5.69 36.52 6.45

Avg 86 6.20 38.12 6.93 86 5.21 48.37 8.99

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.t001

Table 2. Free-spelling performance.Performance rates of all individual participants and the grand average during free-spelling. The table lists the total number of K trials (i.e., including backspaces), total number of K’spelled symbols at the end of the session, number of C correctly spelled symbols, the aver-age time T needed for spelling one symbol (including overhead: inter-trial time), and an estimate of Symbols Per Minute (SPM). Note that only early-stopping was applied, which was trained with a fixed targeted accuracy of 95 percent for all participants. Note that C is an informal measure of correctness.

Early-Stopping

K K C T SPM

# # # sec sym/min

S01 50 16 16 4.75 4.04

S02 50 50 49 4.36 13.47

S03 50 44 43 4.92 10.48

S04 50 50 50 3.93 15.25

S05 50 38 34 5.12 7.97

S06 50 14 10 6.20 1.94

S07 50 40 0 6.20 0.00

S08 40 26 26 4.56 8.56

S09 29 27 27 4.44 12.59

S10 50 26 13 6.11 2.55

S11 35 27 26 4.08 10.94

S12 50 38 38 5.58 8.17

Avg 46 33 28 5.02 8.00

(16)

During debriefing, the participants seemed to be unaware of symbols being encoded by dif-ferent flash patterns. Furthermore, none of the participants reported the stimulation to be annoying. Instead, participants liked participating in the study and were overall amazed by the ability to spell words solely using brain activity. Most participants tried to influence the perfor-mance of the speller by putting more attention to the letter of interest, while ignoring all others. Some participants reported strategies like repeating the symbol of interest in mind.

Validation of reconvolution

Reconvolution can be split into two steps. The first step is estimation, in which pulse responses are derived from the full response. The two pulse responses that are derived from decomposing

the signals according to the structure matrix are shown inFig 9. These resemble a wavelet-like

curve: a modulated sine wave with a constant frequency (between 13 and 15 Hertz) and a par-ticipant-dependent phase. Also note that the difference between the two pulse responses indi-cates an enlargement of the amplitude whenever the underlying impulse gets longer. The amplitude and phase of the pulse responses were not significantly correlated with accuracy.

Fig 9. Pulse responses.The spatially filtered pulse responses derived by the estimation step in reconvolution (left) and corresponding zero-padded power spectra (right) are shown for each participant. The top figures show the pulse responses on a short flash, the bottom ones show those for a long flash. The black bars represent the length of a single flash.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g009

(17)

The second step in reconvolution is generation, in which the estimated pulse responses are superimposed, according to the bit-sequence, and summed to obtain a prediction for the full response. The quality of fit between the original ERP and the predicted ERP for both

self-pre-diction and cross-preself-pre-diction is shown inFig 10. The explained variance is computed by

per-forming 10-fold cross-validation. Recall that data was obtained of two different modulated Gold codes during the practice block. By slicing the single-trials to individual code repetitions, 144 trials are obtained. With 10-fold cross-validation responses were predicted with reconvolu-tion using only 14 trials of 1.05 seconds and ERPs were constructed by averaging 130 trials of 1.05 seconds.

The first approach to estimate the predictive power of the reconvolution model is to predict

the codes’own ERP for each channel (i.e., self-prediction). Averaged over participants, the

channel-wise explained variance of the first code was on average 4% (min 0%, max 28%) and on average 5% (min 0%, max 30%) for the second code. The channels with highest explained variance were Oz, O1, O2, Iz and POz. By applying CCA to the training fold to derive optimal

spatial filters (seeFig 11) and thus to combine channel-wise information, the explained

vari-ance is further optimized to an average of 44% (min 37%, max 66%) for the first code and on average 50% (min 37%, max 66%) for the second code. Here, the importance of spatial filtering is clearly visible, because even the best single-channel is outperformed by spatially filtering.

This only proves the ability to predict an already measured ERP. It is more interesting to test generalizability and to estimate the ability to predict unseen classes. We can infer this by

Fig 10. Grand average responses.Grand averages of both spatially filtered ERPs (solid lines) and predicted responses (dashed lines) are shown. The quality of fit by generating the response to the same bit-sequence as reconvolution was trained on is shown at the top (r2= 0.343). The quality of fit by predicting the response to a bit-sequence that was not used during training is shown at the bottom (r2= 0.476).

(18)

cross-predicting the ERP from each code with the reconvolution of the other. Averaged over participants, the channel-wise explained variance of the cross-predicted first code was on aver-age 1% (min 0%, max 10%), and for the second code on averaver-age 1% (min 0%, max 11%). By applying CCA, the explained variance is further optimized to an average of 44% (min 30%, max 66%) for the cross-predicted first code and on average 50% (min 37%, max 66%) for the second code.

Validation of optimizations

By applying reconvolution on the data from the training block, templates were generated for

responses to each of the 65 modulated Gold codes inV. The cross-correlation matrix of such a

subset contained off-diagonal values between -0.38 and 0.42, indicating that there may be room for optimization in picking a subset. An optimal subset of 36 Platinum codes was com-puted using the clustering technique as outlined above. Among participants different subsets were selected from the set of 65 modulated Gold codes. To illustrate the importance of subset selection, we indicate the ease of confusion (max cross-correlation) of the average of 200 ran-dom subset selections and the optimization algorithm. The optimization significantly lowers

the maximum cross-correlation between different classes (p= 0.001) and the mean correlation

(p= 0.032), it did not significantly affect the minimum correlation (p= 0.051).

Cross-talk may be a problem in BCI performance when responses to non-target neighbour-ing stimuli leak into the signal. To optimize the layout of the grid we allocated those sequences from the Platinum set that have relatively high cross-correlations to non-neighbouring cells of the 6 by 6 matrix, using the layout optimization algorithm outlined above. The pair-wise cross-correlation of neighbours in the layout was on average -0.04 (min -0.32, max 0.09). As baseline layout selection, we simulated a random process of 200 layouts. With respect to this baseline, the layout optimization significantly lowers the maximum correlation between two neighbours

(p<0.001) and the mean correlation (p<0.001). The minimum correlation was not affected

(p= 0.711).

As shown inTable 1, early-stopping improved the time to make a decision without

distor-tion of the accuracy. A paired t-test was performed to test for a significant effect between the ITR achieved by the fixed-length condition and the early-stopping condition. Early-stopping

significantly (p= 0.017) improved the communication speed as compared to fixed-length trials

in the proposed BBVEP-based speller.

Discussion

In this study, a 6 × 6 novel BBVEP-based matrix speller is evaluated. The study extends litera-ture by exploring Gold codes for stimulation, instead of m-sequences. Gold codes enable

Fig 11. Spatial filters.The grand average spatial filterWXis depicted (middle). Based on the correlation with

this grand average a typical (right, i.e. high correlation) and atypical (left, i.e., low correlation)WXare shown

as well.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133797.g011

(19)

asynchronous BBVEP-based BCI, because Gold codes only show a high auto-correlation at time-lag 0. Specifically, a Gold code is uncorrelated with a any shifted version of itself, as well as with any other code. Thus, in case of Gold codes it is not required to know the exact onset of stimulation. Conversely, in case of one circular-shifted m-sequence a phase-lock is required. Alternatively, a set of different m-sequences does not guarantee optimal cross-correlation. Therefore, this study provides the first steps towards an asynchronous BBVEP-based BCI, without relying on a synchronization process. Additionally, a set of Gold codes contains numerous near-orthogonal codes and therefore allows for direct application to BCIs with a higher number of classes.

A set of Gold codes contains numerous codes, so it becomes unfeasible to generate tem-plates by averaging multiple trials to obtain ERPs. Instead, we proposed a linear generative framework, called reconvolution, with which responses can be predicted. The assumption of

linearity is investigated by several other studies (e.g., [17] and [23]). In this study, only two

events were used (short and long flashes). In all, reconvolution allows for short training ses-sions and accurate response-prediction. Specifically, it is shown that reconvolution can explain up to 66% of the variance of spatially filtered ERPs. In this study, reconvolution has been proven to accurately predict responses, because during the testing phase other stimulation sequences were used than during the training phase. These generated responses were well suited to use as templates for a template matching classifier.

A set of Gold codes may contain more codes than is required. We proposed routines to opti-mize the selection and allocation of a subset of codes to the speller grid, given their predicted responses. Additionally, an early stopping method is proposed that dynamically stops trials when a certain confidence is reached. In this study, the confidence was measured as the differ-ence between the best and second best matching template. Whenever the measured margin was greater than the learned margin, the trial was stopped and the classifier emitted the output. Each of these optimizations were shown to significantly increase discriminability and commu-nication rates.

In summary, on a copy-spelling task, twelve participants reached an average ITR of 48 bits per minute and an SPM of 9 symbols per minute. On a free-spelling task, most participants could spell full sentences, reaching an average of 8 symbols per minute. This communication

rate is lower than rates reported in literature (e.g., 144 bits per minute in [11]), but nevertheless

an excellent performance when compared to more common stimulation paradigms (e.g., P300-spellers). We propose several ways with which we could increase the performance achieved so far.

First, we clearly separated training from testing by using two sets of modulated Gold codes. The motivation behind this was to ensure reconvolution predicted responses to novel stimuli, which it clearly did well. In principle, there may have been some over-fitting of the training data causing the performance to remain low during testing. However, the validation of recon-volution clearly shows no over-fitting. Specifically, cross-predicting a bit-sequence resulted in similar percentages of explained variance as for self-prediction.

Second, changing stimuli properties could potentially increase performance. In our study we used Gold codes whereas other studies used one m-sequence. M-sequences have near-zero auto-correlation, meaning time-shifted variants are near-orthogonal. One set of Gold codes contains numerous codes that are optimized to a known three-valued cross-correlation, though this cross-correlation is slightly worse than the auto-correlation function of an m-sequence. Thus, the stimulation sequences explored in this study are less orthogonal and could therefore cause a higher level of interference. However, the accuracies achieved in this study imply a

clear distinction between codes’responses up to 100% correct classifications is possible. In

(20)

low-frequency content and to limit the events to only short and long flashes. Together with the high

stimulation frequency of 120 Hertz this may have affected the brains’response (e.g., due to a

refractory period).

Third, changing the task performed by the participants during the online experiments could potentially increase performance. Participants may have disliked the random copy-spelling task and hence were less motivated to finish the experiment. The more common copy-spelling task in which full natural sentences are spelled, may encourage participants by bringing the task closer to a natural situation; this may increase motivation and attentional resources, con-tributing to higher accuracies. In addition, an inter-trial interval of 2 seconds was used in our

experiment, whereas 0.85 seconds might have been sufficient [11]. Hence, our specific

experi-mental design could be improved for higher accuracies and shorter (inter) trial lengths, which results in a higher ITR.

Fourth, the free-spelling task should best evaluate the practical usage of the system. As is

observable fromTable 2, for four of twelve participants (participants 1, 6, 7, and 11) only a

small part of the selected characters resulted in usable output at the end of the run. For these participants, the majority of selections were either incorrect or used for backspacing. Especially for these participants, but also in general, it remains a trade-off whether or not to utilize a fast but less accurate system, or to improve accuracy by increasing the length of single-trials. It is precisely this objective that an early-stopping algorithm should address dynamically. In this study, the early-stopping algorithm was always calibrated to achieve a 95% targeted accuracy,

which was too optimistic for ten out of twelve participants (seeTable 1). This may have

over-complicated the estimation of the stopping margins, which as a result becomes an inaccurate measure of confidence.

Future research will aim at improving the methods to meet higher communication rates as reached in literature. Additionally, there is much room for extending BBVEP-based BCI to

asynchronous stimulation, other modalities (e.g., [24]), and covert attention (e.g., [25]). Finally,

apart from the speller BCI, a few practical applications have been developed and used to

dem-onstrate the system’s feasibility. One of these replicates the fairground can-toss game, in which

participants’gaze is sufficient to make the cans fall down. In the future, we will also test other

amplifiers to verify if different applications in the consumer range will become feasible, and to

increase the application’s practicality for the end-user (e.g., patients).

Conclusion

In this study we reported a BBVEP-based BCI using novel stimuli, a generative model and rou-tines to optimize the speller design. More specifically, we presented a paradigm for visual stim-ulation with so-called modulated Gold codes. The codes were modulated to limit their low frequency content, which also translates them into sequences of only two basic flashes, a short and a long one. We developed a method, called reconvolution, that generates two transient responses, one for each flash. With these transient responses, an independent set of modulated Gold codes (build up from the same two flashes) was predicted. These predictions formed the templates of a template matching classifier, which classifies a new single-trial by maximizing the correlation with the templates. We used Canonical Correlation Analysis to derive optimal spatial filters. Because the set of codes was larger than the needed number of classes, a Platinum subset was selected in which responses to the codes were best distinguished from each other. The codes in this Platinum subset were then optimally allocated to cells in the 6 × 6 speller matrix to minimize cross-talk from neighbouring cells. The data was sliced into small time intervals to derive an early-stopping criterion based on the margin between the best and second best correlation between single-trials and templates.

(21)

We tested this BCI setup in an online experiment involving two conditions: fixed-length and early-stopping. In both conditions, a correct classification rate of 86% was achieved. How-ever, in terms of communication rates early-stopping was significantly better (30% on average), achieving an average ITR of 48 bits per minute and SPM of 9 symbols per minute.

The availability of a generative model makes it possible to use a short training session and to optimize the performance of a specific BCI setup without testing all stimulus sequences, which is a great advantage. Reconvolution explained on average 50% up to 66% of the explained vari-ance in spatially filtered responses to both seen and unseen bit-sequences. Together with Gold codes and optimization schemes, a pipeline is provided that can easily be extended to numer-ous classes and can directly be applied to asynchronnumer-ous BCI. Both these challenges may be infeasible when m-sequences (i.e., the requirement of a phase-lock) and non-generative meth-ods (i.e., the requirement of long training sessions) are employed.

To conclude, we have shown the importance and performance of a BBVEP-based BCI using Gold codes, a generative model, and several optimization routines, thereby enabling (or restor-ing) communication and control by brain activity only.

Acknowledgments

We thank Iris Hulzink for her assistance during the EEG experiments. In addition, we would like to thank Marcel van Gerven, Jan-Matthijs Schoffelen and Kelly Woudsma for proof-read-ing the manuscript. We gratefully acknowledge the support of the BrainGain Smart Mix Pro-gram of the Netherlands Ministry of Economic Affairs and the Netherlands Ministry of Education, Culture and Science.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JT PD. Performed the experiments: JT. Analyzed the data: JT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JT PD JF PB. Wrote the paper: JT PD JF.

References

1. van Gerven M, Farquhar J, Schaefer R, Vlek R, Geuze J, Nijholt A, et al. The brain-computer interface cycle. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2009; 6(4):041001. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/6/4/041001PMID:

19622847

2. Farwell LA, Donchin E. Talking off the top of your head: toward a mental prosthesis utilizing event-related brain potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1988; 70(6):510–523. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(88)90149-6PMID:2461285

3. Gao S, Wang Y, Gao X, Hong B. Visual and auditory brain-computer interfaces. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 2014; 61(5):1436–1447. doi:10.1109/TBME.2014.2300164PMID:24759277

4. Treder MS, Blankertz B. Research covert attention and visual speller design in an ERP-based brain-computer interface. Behavioral & Brain Functions. 2010; 6.

5. Volosyak I. SSVEP-based Bremen-BCI interface—boosting information transfer rates. Journal of Neu-ral Engineering. 2011; 8(3):036020. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/8/3/036020PMID:21555847

6. Bin G, Gao X, Wang Y, Hong B, Gao S. VEP-based brain-computer interfaces: time, frequency, and code modulations [Research Frontier]. Computational Intelligence Magazine, IEEE. 2009; 4(4):22–26. doi:10.1109/MCI.2009.934562

7. Sutter EE. The visual evoked response as a communication channel. In: Proceedings of the IEEE Sym-posium on Biosensors; 1984:95–100.

8. Sutter EE. The brain response interface: communication through visually-induced electrical brain responses. Journal of Microcomputer Applications. 1992; 15(1):31–45. doi:10.1016/0745-7138(92) 90045-7

(22)

10. Spüler M, Rosenstiel W, Bogdan M. One class SVM and canonical correlation analysis increase perfor-mance in a c-VEP based brain-computer interface (BCI). In: Proceedings of 20th European Symposium on Artificial Neural Networks (ESANN 2012). Bruges, Belgium; 2012:103–108.

11. Spüler M, Rosenstiel W, Bogdan M. Online adaptation of a c-VEP brain-computer interface (BCI) based on error-related potentials and unsupervised learning. PloS ONE. 2012; 7(12):e51077. doi:10. 1371/journal.pone.0051077PMID:23236433

12. Gold R. Optimal binary sequences for spread spectrum multi-plexing. IEEE Transactions on Informa-tion Theory. 1967; 13:619–621. doi:10.1109/TIT.1967.1054048

13. Desain P, Farquhar J. Method for processing a brain wave signal and brain computer interface. US Pat-ent US20110251511. 2009.

14. Desain P, Thielen J, van den Broek, P, Farquhar, J. Brain computer interface using broadband evoked potentials. NL Patent 2013245. 2014.

15. Desain P, Thielen J. Broad-Band Visually Evoked Potentials: Re(con)volution in Brain-Computer Inter-facing. DANS. 2015.

16. Golomb SW, Welch LR, Goldstein RM, Hales AW. Shift register sequences. Aegean Park Press Laguna Hills, CA; 1982; 78.

17. Capilla A, Pazo-Alvarez P, Darriba A, Campo P, Gross J. Steady-state visual evoked potentials can be explained by temporal superposition of transient event-related responses. PloS ONE. 2011; 6(1): e14543. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0014543PMID:21267081

18. Desain P, Farquhar J, Blankespoor J, Gielen S. Detecting spread spectrum pseudo random noise tags in EEG/MEG using a structure-based decomposition. 4th Int BCI Workshop and Training Course 2008. 2008. Graz, Austria.

19. Tan PN, Steinbach M, Kumar V. Introduction to data mining. Boston; Pearson Addison Wesley. 2006.

20. Schreuder M, Höhne J, Blankertz B, Haufe S, Dickhaus T, Tangermann M. Optimizing event-related potential based brain-computer interfaces: a systematic evaluation of dynamic stopping methods. Jour-nal of Neural Engineering. 2013; 10(3):036025. doi:10.1088/1741-2560/10/3/036025PMID:23685458

21. Wolpaw JR, Ramoser H, McFarland DJ, Pfurtscheller G. EEG-based communication: improved accu-racy by response verification. Rehabilitation Engineering, IEEE Transactions on. 1998; 6(3):326–333. doi:10.1109/86.712231

22. Schreuder, M and Hohne, J and Treder, M and Blankertz, B and Tangermann, M. Performance optimi-zation of ERP-based BCIs using dynamic stopping. Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. EMBC. 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE. 2011;4580–4583.

23. Lalor EC, Pearlmutter BA, Reilly RB, McDarby G, Foxe JJ. The VESPA: a method for the rapid estima-tion of a visual evoked potential. NeuroImage. 2006; 32(4):1549–1561. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage. 2006.05.054PMID:16875844

24. Farquhar J, Blankespoor J, Vlek R, Desain P. Towards a noise-tagging auditory BCI-paradigm. In Pro-ceedings of the 4th Int BCI Workshop and Training Course 2008. 2008;50–55. Graz, Austria.

25. Kelly SP, Lalor EC, Reilly RB, Foxe JJ. Visual spatial attention tracking using high-density SSVEP data for independent brain-computer communication. Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering, IEEE Transactions on. 2005; 13(2):172–178. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2005.847369

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Se o Rei não tivesse deixado em branco um espaço suficiente para as duas ou três linhas do autógrafo de encerramento, Fernão de Pina não se coibia de o escrever no lugar

Em todas as outras amostras (7) verificou-se que a pesquisa de esporos de clostrídios sulfito-redutores foi negativa em 10' 1 g(&lt;10/g). Nas restantes 5 amostras não se

Quando a renda líquida do estabelecimento, no curto e longo prazo, é positiva e a renda líquida do capitalista também o é (ele não está endivi- dado), as decisões são de

(2015) critically reviewed the chronology of hand stencils in the context of new U-series minimum ages for stencils in El Castillo, concluding on the basis of production technique

In the context of the P3M project (Professional Practices of Teachers of Mathematics), we are developing multimedia cases for teacher education that integrate video and

Hodiernamente para melhorar a qualidade do ensino público no Brasil especificamente na educação básica é preciso uma emenda à constituição federal de 1988, nos investimentos da

[r]

Estes factores contemplam a generalidade dos aspectos relacionados com a segurança contra incêndio em edifícios antigos, desde o início do incêndio, o seu desenvolvimento e