• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Social competence: an evolutionary approach

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Share "Social competence: an evolutionary approach"

Copied!
10
0
0

Texto

(1)

Social

competence:

an

evolutionary

approach

Barbara

Taborsky

1

and

Rui

F.

Oliveira

2,3

1

BehaviouralEcology,InstituteofEcologyandEvolution,UniversityofBern,Wohlenstrasse50A, CH-3032Hinterkappelen,Switzerland

2InstitutoSuperiordePsicologiaAplicada,UnidadedeInvestigac¸a˜oemEco-Etologia,RuaJardimdoTabaco34, 1149-041Lisboa,Portugal

3

ChampalimaudNeuroscienceProgramme,InstitutoGulbenkiandeCieˆncia,RuadaQuintaGrande6,2780-156Oeiras,Portugal

‘Socialcompetence’referstotheabilityofanindividual tooptimiseitssocialbehaviourdependingonavailable social information.Although suchability will enhance socialinteractions andthusraise Darwinianfitness,its evolutionaryandecologicalsignificancehasbeenlargely ignored. Social competence is based on behavioural flexibility.We proposethat the studyof social compe-tence requires an integrative approach that aims to understandhowthebraintranslatessocialinformation intoflexiblebehaviouralresponses,howflexibilitymight beconstrainedbythedevelopmentalhistoryofan indi-vidual or by trade-offs with other (ecological) compe-tences,andhowsocialplasticity feedsbackonfitness. Finallywe proposeahypothesis of howsocial compe-tencecanbecomeadriverofsocialevolution.

Behaviouralflexibilityasphenotypicplasticity

Adaptationtotheenvironmentisauniversal characteris-tic of living systems. According to classic evolutionary theory,adaptationbynaturalselectionreliesonheritable phenotypicvariationproducedbygeneticvariation. How-ever,whentherateofgeneticevolutionarychangeis out-pacedbychangesintheenvironmenttheneedforadaptive change without genetic mutation emerges [1]. In this scenario,theevolutionofphenotypicplasticityisfavoured, thatis,acertaingenotypeproducesdifferentphenotypes depending on environmental conditions [2]. Among ani-mals,behavioural traitsexhibit agreaterplasticity than morphologicalandphysiologicaltraitsandplasticchanges arereversiblewithinanindividual’slifetime(‘behavioural flexibility’).Thismakesbehaviouralflexibilityapowerful, immediate mechanism allowing organisms to adapt to changing environmental conditions, which may or may notbefollowedbyotherflexibleadjustmentsofphysiology ormorphology.Manyoftheseresponsesaresimplereflexes and fixed action patterns elicited by a stimulus in the environment,whenitdeterministicallypredictsan appro-priateresponse.However,whenenvironmentalcomplexity andvariabilityincrease,thecapacitytoadaptivelymodify behaviourasafunctionofexperienceandcontextis need-ed. Although some degree of context-dependent beha-vioural flexibility may be achieved with genetically determinedrules,behaviouralflexibilitywilloftendepend

oncognitiveabilities(understoodastheacquisition, reten-tion,anduseofinformation;[3])thatallowindividualsto adapt behavioural outputtospecific situationsina com-plexandvariableworld(e.g.,see[4]).

Interestingly, the evolutionary study of behavioural flexibility has rarely been framed within the scope of phenotypic plasticity, but rather in terms of cognitive evolution andecology[3,5].Thisismostprobablydueto thefactthatincontrasttomorphologicalandlifehistory traits (which have been the main focus of phenotypic plasticity studies,and whoseplasticity results from pro-cessesduringdevelopmentandisusuallynon-reversible) behavioural flexibility involves rapid changes, is labile, and ispresent during the whole life ofthe animal[6,7].

Glossary

Behaviouralreactionnorm(BRN):thesetofbehaviouralphenotypesthata singleindividualproducesinagivensetofenvironments[8].Thisisincontrast to‘reactionnorms’describingtypicallyirreversible,developmentalplasticityof asinglegenotype.ThusBRNsdescribefastresponses(withinalifetime)byan individualtovariationintheenvironment.

Developmentalplasticity:variationinthetraitsofindividualsthatresultsfrom processesduringdevelopmentasaconsequenceofenvironmentalvariation andthatistypicallyirreversible[6].

Epigeneticmodifications:changes ingenefunctionthatdo notinvolve a change in the coding-sequence. Examples of such changes are DNA methylationandhistoneacetylation,bothofwhichmodifygenome-regional geneexpressionwithoutchangingthesequenceoftheaffectedgenes. Immediateearlygenes(IEGs):genesthatshowrapidandtransientexpression inresponsetoawidevarietyofextra-cellularstimuliandintheabsenceofde novoproteinsynthesis.Theirproductsactas transcriptionfactors onlate responsegenesoraseffectorsthatchangethefunctioningofthecell,andthus theyrepresentthefirstgenomicresponsetostimuli[60].

Neuralplasticity:structuralandfunctionalchangesofthenervoussystemasa result of inputfrom the environment.Two major mechanisms of neural plasticityoperateatdifferenttimescales:structuralrewiringofneuralcircuits isslowandlong-lastingandinducesdramaticbehaviouralchanges,whereas biochemicalmodulationofexistingneuralnetworksispostulatedtomediate fastandtransientchangesbetweenmotivationalstatesthatpromotegradual changesinbehaviouralexpression.

Neurogenomicstate:transcriptomeprofileofthebrainareasofinterestfora givenbehaviouralstate(e.g.,expressionofagivensocialphenotype). Phenotypic(behavioural)flexibility:changesofthe(behavioural)phenotype thatcanbereversedwithinanindividual’slifetime[6].

Phenotypic(behavioural)plasticity: anumbrella termsubsuming different classesofplasticity,includingdevelopmentalplasticity,phenotypicflexibility, andlife-cyclestaging[6].

Socialcompetence:theabilityofananimaltooptimisetheexpressionofits socialbehaviourasafunctionoftheavailablesocialinformation.

Socialcontext:anysocialstimulusthatcanvaryacrossagradient(e.g.,group composition,offspringbegging,orcompetitordensity)[8].

Socialinformation:anyinformationthatisgenerated bythebehaviourof anotherorganism.

Correspondingauthor:Taborsky,B. (barbara.taborsky@iee.unbe.ch).

(2)

However,aphenotypicplasticityapproachtobehavioural flexibilitywouldprovideanewandpowerfulframeworkto understand theadaptive nature andevolution ofanimal behaviour; namely by introducing the concept of ‘beha-vioural reaction norms’ (BRN; seeGlossary) as a toolto visualiseandquantitativelyanalyseindividualplasticity, andtounravelindividualbyenvironment(IE) interac-tionsunderlyingit[8].Forexample,recentlythisapproach facilitated the establishment of a single framework to integratethestudyofanimalpersonalityandbehavioural plasticity, two phenomena usually studied separately, thereby enhancing the understanding of their adaptive significance [8]. This approach will also help to unravel the proximate mechanisms of behavioural flexibility, by extending the framework used to study the causes of phenotypicplasticity tobehaviour(e.g.,[2,9]).

Adaptivebehaviouralflexibilityinthesocialdomain The social domain is arguably the most complex and fluctuating component of an animal’s environment as it involves interaction with other behavioural agents with inherentlyassociatedhigherlevelsofunpredictability.An animal interacting with its non-social abiotic or biotic environment willoftenmodifythisenvironment,thereby creatingecologicalfeedbackontheindividualitselfforcing ittoflexiblyadjustitsbehaviour(e.g.,aforagingindividual changesthelocalresourcedensityenforcinganadjustment ofsubsequentforagingdecisions).Suchecologicalfeedback isubiquitousandcanevenspangenerations(e.g.,[10]),but neverthelessthedegreesoffreedomofthisfeedbackwillbe finite.Incontrast,feedbackinthesocialdomaincanhave nearlyinfinitedegreesoffreedom,asfeedbackwilldepend notonlyonsocialcontextbutalsoonthenumber,identity, andtheexternal(e.g.,rankandsize)andinternalstatesof theagentsthatsocialbehaviourisdirectedtowards. Con-sequently, dealing with social complexity requires the evolutionofcognitivemechanismsthatallowthe individ-ualtoassesstheinternal(‘emotional’)stateofother organ-ismsandthesocialcontext,andtointegrateandprocess thesestimulinotjustasaresultofdirecteffectsof percep-tual information, but rather as a function of what that perceptual information means to the individual at that moment in time [11]. Therefore, social decision-making depends on some kind of social experiential knowledge thatallowsorganismstoevaluatestimuliandtodetermine theappropriatebehaviour.Thus,morethaninanyother behavioural domain it is expected that social behaviour shouldexhibit highlevelsofplasticity.

Theabilityofindividualstoregulatetheexpressionof their social behaviour in order to optimise their social relationships is referred to as ‘social competence’. It shouldbestressedherethatflexibilityintheexpression ofsocialbehaviourisnecessarybutnotsufficientforsocial competence,sincethelatterimpliesnotonlyvariationin the response to the same social stimuli depending on additionalsocialinformation,butalsothedemonstration that this variation is adaptive (i.e., optimises the response).Socialcompetenceallowsorganismstoexpress appropriate responses to demandsand togenerate and capitaliseonopportunitiesinthesocialenvironment[12]

therebyultimatelyenhancingtheirfitness.Forexample,

socialcompetencewillenableindividualstoavoid engag-ingincostlysocialinteractionsorbeingejectedfromtheir socialgroups.

Social competence has been shown to influence the performanceofdifferentsocialbehavioursacrossdifferent socialcontexts (Box 1). Thereare two possible scenarios thatcanexplainthisjointeffect.Eithersocialcompetence iscomposed ofacollection ofsocialskills, each ofwhich evolvesratherindependently,or,alternatively,social com-petence may be an ability underlying all social sub-domains(e.g.,interactionswithcompetitors,mating,and socialforaging)resultinginpositivewithin-individual cor-relations of performance across different social contexts (Figure 1). Although some evidence supports the former hypothesis(e.g.,independenceofsocialskillsthat contrib-ute tomating successin cowbirds, suchas song quality, courtshippersistence,andcompetitiveability[13])mostof the available evidence supports the second alternative, namelythe factthat thesocialenvironment experienced duringontogenycanaffecttheappropriatenessofsuitesof social behaviours belonging to various different social contexts[14–17].

Animalsocialcompetence

Todatethestudyofsocialcompetencehasbeenmainlya domainofthesocialsciences,withastrongfocusoncausal relationshipsbetweensocial factorsand thedevelopment andexpressionofsocialcompetenceinhumans[12,18,19]. However, indications of social competence are also well known from non-human animals. Interacting animals respond to the presence of bystanders (‘audience effect’) bychangingtheirsignallingbehaviouraccordingtothetype ofaudienceand socialcontext (e.g.,[20,21]). Bycontrast, bystandersextractinformationfromobservedinteractions (‘eavesdropping’)thattheyuseinsubsequentinteractions

Evolving neurogenomic machinery underlying social plascity

Social competence Selecon Territory defence Mang Social foraging Fitness

TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Figure1.Socialcompetenceishypothesisedtobeanabilityunderlyingallsocial behaviourthatiscontingentonsocialinformationandthustoaffectbehaviourin manyorallsocialcontexts.Thisshould giverisetopositivewithin-individual correlations of social performance across different social contexts; social performanceisunderpositiveselectionandaffectsfitness.Differentlycoloured rectanglesrepresentdifferentindividuals;thewidthoftheserectanglesrepresents anindividual’ssocialperformanceinthedifferentsocialcontexts.

Opinion

TrendsinEcologyandEvolution December2012,Vol.27,No.12

(3)

Box1.Howtomeasuresocialcompetence

Ifwewanttostudyindividualvariationinsocialcompetence,weneed anapproachthatcapturestheuniversalnatureofthistrait.Wepropose todecide upon arepresentative, ecologically relevantsetof social situations, of which we understand the adequacy of the possible involvedbehaviours.Weshouldexposeindividualstothesesituations instandardisedtrialstestingthem:(i)indifferentsocialcontexts(e.g., dominance relationships, mating, and brood care) to test for the presenceofageneralsocialability;(ii)indifferentsocialroles(e.g., dominantandsubordinate)withinthesamecontexttotestifthisability isbasedon plasticity;(iii)in anunknown socialsituation(e.g., an ecologically relevant situation that had not yet been encountered duringontogeny)tocheckfortheabilitytogeneraliseacrosssocial situations;(iv)atdifferentlifestagestotestifindividualdifferencesin socialcompetencepersistovertime.Itisimportanttohaveapriori predictionsaboutwhichsocialbehaviourswillbeoptimalinagiven test. Classifying those behaviours as socially competent that are expressedbyindividualswiththehighestDarwinianfitnessmaybe misleading, because a high fitness may result from competences outsidethesocialdomain(e.g.,solitaryforagingability).

We illustrate this research agenda by an example from the cooperatively breeding cichlid Neolamprologus pulcher (Figure I).

Thesefishhadbeenrearedeither withorwithoutthepresenceof older conspecifics to investigate whether experiencing a more complex social environment results in better social competence [16,17].Threechallengetests(T1–T3)wereconductedtoassesssocial competencealong thefourabove mentionedaxes(TableI).InT1 juvenileswereassignedeithertheownershipofacriticalresource(a shelter) or of an intruder (asymmetric competition). In nature, subordinate group members defend private shelters within the breeders’territoryagainstothergroupmembers.Shelteraccesscan decideoversurvivalaspredationpressureisintense.Theintruder rolereflectsthesituationofa juvenilesubordinateinsearchofan ownprivateshelterafteritisnolongerallowedtoaccessthenatal breedingcavity.T2simulatedasymmetriccontestoverashelter,in which both opponents had been assigned therole ofthe shelter ownerpriortothetest.Thismimicsasituationinwhichashelterhas to be defended against an at least equally motivated opponent (symmetriccompetition).InT3larger,adultsubordinateswereforced toachieveacceptanceattheterritoryofanunfamiliarbreederpair,a situationfishwouldencounterafterdispersalfromthenatalterritory [74].In thissituation subordinates shouldstrive foracceptance as broodcarehelpers.

(a) (b) 0 0.4 0.8 1.2 1.6 * * T1: Asymmetric compeon * * *

T2: Symmetric compeon T3: Integraon in social group

Shelter owner Intruder Winner Loser Subordinate

20 20 25 10 10 15 15 0 0 2 4 6 8 5 +F -F+F -F +F -F

Threat display Submission Open aggression Submission Submission at breeders' shelter

+F -F +F -F

0 5

TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution FigureI.(a)Neolamprologuspulcherthathadbeenrearedtogetherwitholderconspecificsshowedmoreoftheexpectedappropriatebehavioursinanasymmetric competitiontest(blue),asymmetriccompetitiontest(red),andapreviouslyunknownsocialchallenge,namelybeingexposedtoanunfamiliarbreederpair(green).(b) Neolamprologuspulcherlivesandbreedsinsocialgroupsof3–38individualsthatarestructuredbyasize-dependentdominancehierarchy.Relatedandunrelated smallerindividualshelpthedominantbreederpairrearingtheiroffspring.

TableI.ThreebehaviouralteststoassessthedifferencesinsocialcompetencebetweenN.pulcherrearedwithorwithoutolder conspecifics(parentsand/orhelpers).Inalltests,fishrearedwitholderconspecificsshowedsignificantlymoreofthebehaviours predictedtobeappropriateinagivensituation(FigureI),whereasnosignificantdifferenceswerefoundinbehaviourspredictedto beinappropriate[16,17]

Challengetest (i)Socialcontext (ii)Differentroles withinacontext

(iii)Isthe situation known?

(iv)Lifestage Predicted appropriate behaviour

Predicted inappropriate behaviour

T1 Competition Owner Yes Earlyjuvenile Threatdisplay Openaggression,

submission

Intruder Submission Escape

T2 Competition Winner Yes Latejuvenile Openaggression Threatdisplay,

submission

Loser Submission Escape

T3 Integrationby

subordinate inanewgroup

- No Adult Submissionnear

prospectivebreeding cavity

Aggression,escape

(4)

withtheobservedindividuals[22,23].Familiaritywithan opponentisalsoknowntoreduce(‘dearenemyeffect’[24]) orincrease(‘nastyneighboureffect’[25])fighting behav-iourdependingontherelativethreatimposedtoterritory holders byneighboursversus strangers.Previous social experiencecanaffectsubsequentbehaviourasinthecase of ‘winner–loser effects’ demonstrated across different animal taxa [26,27], where previous winners are more likely to win successive contests, and losers will more likelyloseagainevenagainstdifferentopponents.A simi-lar effect is known from a cooperative context where individualsgivemore helptosocialpartnersiftheyhad receivedhelpbyanotherconspecificbefore(‘generalised reciprocity’ [28]).Experimentshave shownthatNorway ratsoptimisetheirsocialbehaviourasafunctionofsocial informationobtainedinpreviousfights,asacting contin-gently on this information increased an actor’s fitness. Previous winners won successive fights after a shorter time despiteareduced amountofaggressivebehaviour, and losers faced a decreased risk of injury despite a reducedamountofsubmissivebehaviourshown[29].Most of the abovementioned effects of social information on behaviourhavebeenobservedtobewidespreadinnatural populations [24,25,27,30] and their existence was confirmedbytargetedfieldexperiments[21,24–27].Thus we propose that social competence allows animals to efficiently navigatethe complexitiesoftheir social envi-ronment in order tosurvive, reproduce, andraise their offspring,andthereforeitshouldbeseenasakey deter-minantoftheDarwinianfitnessofindividuals.

Socialcompetenceversusgeneral cognition

Sincesocialcompetencecangiverisetoconsistent expres-sionofappropriateflexiblebehaviouralresponsesacross differentsocialcontexts,itappearssimilartotheconcept of general intelligence. In humans, performance on di-versecognitivetasksshowsrobustpositivecorrelations, sothatindividualsscoringhighononecognitiveabilityare alsolikelytoscorewellinothers.Thispositivemanifoldof correlations has been interpreted as being caused by a latentsinglefactornamedgeneralintelligence,orsimply ‘g’ [31]. In comparative cognition it has been debated whethercognitionisorganisedintoseparatemodules or whether there is a generalproblem-solving ability that pertainsallbehaviouraldomainsandecologicaldemands. Although comparative analyses, mainly focused on pri-matesandbirds,suggestthelackofcognitivemodulesand support a generalintelligence scenario [32–35], experi-mental approaches in single speciesproduce conflicting results.Differentdevelopmentalstudiesshowthatearly social experiencecanpromotesocialcompetenceand so-cial learning, but leaves the performance in non-social cognitivetasks unaffected. For example,aftermaternal deprivation,laboratoryratsperformedmorepoorlythan normally-rearedratsinthreesociallearningtasks, where-as in two spatial learning tasks the treatment groups performed equally well [36]. Similarly, communally-reared micehadabettersocialcompetenceduring their adultlifethansingle-motherrearedmice,buttheydidnot differ in spatial learning and memory capacity tests

[37,38]. Zebrafish reared among more diverse social

partners(mixedstrains versussingle strain)weremore aggressivelaterinlife,butnon-socialbehaviourssuchas generalactivity, predator evasionbehaviour andstress recoverywereunaffected[39].Finally,inhumans intelli-gence quotients (IQ) and social intelligence tests for assessment of the theory of mind were not correlated

[40].Partofthesecontradictoryresultsmaybeexplained by the interpretation of comparative studies that have identified a species-level composite general intelligence factor.Asforhumanintelligence,itisimportantto distin-guishbetween‘g’asageneralfirst-orderfactorordominant eigenvalueinafactoranalysis,from‘g’asapsychological constructthatreducescognitiveabilitiestoasingle quanti-tative cognitive or biological process [41]. Alternative models havebeen proposedthatillustratehowa psycho-metric‘g’canbeexplainedbydynamicinteractionsbetween cognitive processes during development, rather than by a single underlying process [41]. Together, these results suggestthatitisunlikelythatsocialcompetenceisa sub-domain of general intelligence, but further studies are neededtoclarifythisissue.

Anevolutionaryframeworkforthestudyofsocial competence

If we understand social competence as a generalability affectingindividualperformanceinasocialenvironment, itshould havethekey propertiesofan ecological perfor-mancetrait[42],namely(i)theexistenceof inter-individ-ualvariationinperformancerangingfromlowtohigh,and (ii) this variation should be positively correlated with fitness[43]. Furthermore, forit tobe an evolvabletrait, potential evolutionary costs, benefits, and trade-offs of socialcompetenceshouldalsobeidentified.

(i) Variationinperformance

Persistentindividualvariationinsocialperformance canbeduetogeneticdispositionoritcanbeinduced duringontogeny.Geneticdispositionhassofarbeen addressedonlyinhumans:twinstudieshavedetected significant heritability of social competence as assessed by questionnaires ([44,45] and references therein).Amuchlargerbodyofliteraturehasshown that variation in socialcompetencecan be environ-mentallyinducedandoriginatesfromdevelopmental plasticity (Table 1). However, only a few of these studiestestedperformanceacrossdifferentcontexts. For example, communally-reared mice exposed to moresocialcontactsshowedmoreappropriatesocial behavioursinthecontextsofdominanceinteractions and brood care, compared to single-mother reared mice[15].Also,cichlidfish(Neolamprologuspulcher) rearedbyalloparentsbehavedmoreappropriatelyin a competitive context or when striving for the acceptance as helper by a breeder pair, than individuals reared among siblings only (Box 1,

[16,17]).

(ii) Correlationwithfitness

Several studies, which had induced differences in social performance by manipulations of social cues during development,foundassociateddifferencesin fitness correlates (Table 1). For example, in some rodents, being exposed to a more complex social

Opinion

TrendsinEcologyandEvolution December2012,Vol.27,No.12

(5)

Table 1. Summary of studies in which (i) the social experience during ontogeny was manipulated, (ii) challenge tests were conducted after the manipulations to test for induced differences in social performance, and (iii) effects of differential social behaviour on fitness correlates were reported

Species Rearing environment Life stage of

experience

Social context of challenge test

Phenotypic effecta Effects on fitnessa Refs

Neolamprologus pulcher Parents and siblings vs siblings-only

Dependent young Competition More appropriate use of aggression

and submission

Shorter fights [16]

Competition More appropriate use of aggression

and submission

Losers less often evicted [17]

Integration in social group

More submissive behaviour at breeding cavities

Higher tolerance by breeders [17]

Zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata

With vs without males present in breeding colony

Dependent young Mate choice, mating Prefer opposite sex over same sex Higher mating success [79]

Cowbirds, Molothrus ater Dynamic vs static flock Adult males Mate choice, mating Larger and more variable ‘singing network’ Higher mating success [13]

Lab mouse, CD1(ICR) Communal vs single-mother

rearing

Dependent young Dominance

interactions

Higher propensity to interact socially Shorter time to find hierarchy position

[14]

Lab mouse, CD1(ICR) Synchronous vs asynchronous

birth-spacing in communal nest

Dependent young Dominance

interactions

Higher aggression/lower affiliative behaviour Shorter time to find hierarchy position

[80]

Lab mouse, CD1(ICR) Mixed sex vs female only litter Dependent young Maternal care Shorter pup retrieval latency,

higher aggression towards intruder males

Better maternal care [81]

Lab mouse, Balb/c Communal vs single-mother

rearing

Dependent young Maternal care Shorter pup retrieval latency, more care,

better nest quality

Better maternal care, F2 had larger litters

[15] Dominance

interactions

Lower aggression towards intruder males

Lab rat, Porton Mixed sex vs female only litter Dependent young Maternal care More elaborate nests and more building

behaviour

Fewer and smaller litters produced [82]

Lab rat, Sprague–Dawley With vs without mother present Dependent young 3 social learning tests Enhanced social learning ability [36]

Short separation from pups

Shorter latency to show maternal care Better maternal care

Oldfield mouse, Peromyscus polionotus

With vs without younger-sibling pups

Subadult daughters Mating, reproduction Better nest quality Higher reproductive success [46]

Rhesus macaque, Macaca mulatta

With vs without mother present Dependent young Staging intra-group conflict

Better in acquisition of dominance rank Higher rank achieved [83]

aDirections of the reported results refer to the respective treatment group that was exposed to the more complex social condition, and that is the first condition mentioned in the column ‘Rearing environment’.

Trends in Ecology and Evolution December 2012, V ol. 27, No . 12 683

(6)

environment during ontogeny resulted in better maternal care behaviour and higher reproductive successforfemalesortheirdaughters[15,46]. (iii) Benefitsofsocialcompetence

Expressingabettersocialcompetenceshould gener-allybebeneficialinallkindsofsocialencounters,and themajorityofanimalspecieswillbeinvolvedinat leastsomesocialencounters.Theexpectedbenefitsof social competence will be particularly strong in sociallylivingspecies,wherebehavioursinvolvedin vital functions such as acquiring and defending resources, predator evasion, and rearing offspring willusuallyinvolvesocialinteractionsand/oroccurin socially heterogeneous environments(e.g., in envir-onments where individuals face quickly changing socialcontextsandadiversearrayofsocial interac-tionsandrelationships).

(iv) Costsofsocialcompetence

There will be costs associated with developing and maintaining the sensory and neural machinery necessary for processing social information that influencessocialbehaviour[47].Arguably,the cogni-tivedemandsofasociallifeselectedforlargerbrains that are metabolically expensive (i.e., social brain hypothesis[48,49]),althoughbrainsizeappearstobe associatedwithquantitativeratherthanqualitative improvements in cognitive abilities [50]. Learning andmemory capacity involved in the correct inter-pretationofsocialinformationwillofteninvolveboth timeandenergy costs[51,52].Additionally, produc-tion costs may arise when costly behaviours are involved in a socially competent response [47]. For example,theappropriateresponsetowardsagonistic displaysbydominantsofthecichlidfishN.pulcheris a submissive displayby subordinates, whichraises the energy expenditure by roughly five times the standardmetabolicrate[53].

(v) Possibletrade-offs

Given the set of possible costs associated with acquiring and maintaining social competence, trade-offs with other body functions are expected on various levels. Obtaining social information needed for an accurate social response increases sampling time and thus compromises the speed of socialdecision-making,especiallyundernoisy condi-tions (i.e.,speed-accuracy trade-offin decision mak-ing[54]).Timeneededduringearlyontogenytolearn interpreting social stimuli and expressing social behaviourcorrectly[16]mightconflictwiththetime required to learn about other important ecological stimuli(shelters,predatorevasionskills,orforaging techniques). Finally, genetic trade-offs may arise fromaninvolvementoflearningintheacquisitionof socialcompetence,assuggestedbytheexistenceofa genetic trade-offbetween learning ability and com-petitiveabilityinfruitflylarvae[55,56].

Integratingproximatemechanismsintheevolutionary studyof socialcompetence

Theideathatsocialcompetence–likeanyotheradaptive behaviour–reliesonoptimalbehaviouralrulesis unreal-istic, since its proximate mechanisms will impose con-straints and limits to flexible behavioural responses

[57,58].Being aplastictrait, socialcompetencerelieson thegenerationofmultiplesocialphenotypesfromthesame genotype,aprocessthat isexpectedtoresult from inter-actions between genetic, environmental, and epigenetic processes that lead to neural and behavioural plasticity

[2]. This involves the regulation of genome-wide gene expression in the nervous system (i.e., brain transcrip-tome) by social information, which can be achieved by twodifferentmechanismsatdifferenttimescales. Epige-netic modifications (e.g., DNA methylation and histone

Social environment Neurogenomic State of the BSBN Neural and behavioural plascity E1 E2 En N1 N2 N3 NK Immediate early genes B1 B2 Fitness IEG1 IEG2

TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution

Figure2.Pathdiagramshowingtheproposedrelationshipbetweenthesocialenvironment(E1–En),immediateearlygenes(IEG1andIEG2),neurogenomicstatesof relevantneuralcircuits(N1–Nk),thatwilldeterminetheneuralandbehaviouralplasticitythatinturntranslatesintheexpressionofaspecificbehaviouralprofile(B1and B2),withimplicationsfortheDarwinianfitnessoftheanimal.VariationsinthesocialenvironmentinducechangesinIEGexpressionthatwillorchestratetheplasticityofthe neuralcircuitsunderlyingsocialbehaviourthathavefitnessconsequences.

Opinion

TrendsinEcologyandEvolution December2012,Vol.27,No.12

(7)

modifications)provideawayforenvironmentaleffectsto have a sustained effect on gene expression profile, and therefore they can explain life stage changes in social plasticity as well as inter-individual differences (e.g., ‘behaviouralprofiles’)resultingfromearlylifeconditions, whereasimmediate-earlygene(IEG)responseaccountsfor

short-term changes in neurogenomic states underlying behavioural flexibility [9,59]. IEG (e.g., c-fos, egr-1, and arc) expression does not require the activation of any precedinggene,andsinceIEGsmodifysynapticstructure andfunction,theyrepresenttheearliestgenomicresponse to an inducing stimulus that orchestrates integrated

Box2.Molecularmechanismsofadaptivebehaviouralflexibility Attheproximatelevelbehaviouralflexibilityisachievedbyrewiring orbybiochemicallyswitchingnodesoftheneuralnetworkunderlying socialbehaviour(i.e.,‘BrainSocialBehaviourNetwork’orBSBN[75]) inresponsetoperceivedsocialinformation(FigureI).Therefore,at the molecular level, it depends on the social regulation of gene expression so that different neurogenomic states correspond to differentbehaviouralresponsesandtheswitchesbetweenstatesare orchestratedbysignallingpathwaysthatinterfacethesocial environ-mentandthegenotype.Transientsocially-drivenneuroplasticitycan beachievedbythreedifferentneuronal-activitydependent mechan-isms[2,76]:(a)activation(e.g.,phosphorylation)ofproteinsthatthen act as transcription factors for IEGs (e.g., CREB) or for delayed responsegenes(DRGs)orregulateintracellularsignallingpathways (e.g., MAPKs); (b)neuronal activity-dependent transcription factors (e.g.,pCREB)activateimmediateearlygenes(IEG)thatcanencode othertranscriptionfactors(e.g.,c-FosandEgr-1)orsynapticproteins (Arcand Homer1a), henceacting asneuromolecular switchesthat change the expressionof co-regulatedgene sets inthe brain; (c) transcription of microRNAs that regulate translation of synaptic proteins (e.g., miR-134). By contrast, socially-driven long-lasting

changesin socialbehaviourrelyonepigeneticmodifications (e.g., DNAmethylation andhistone modifications) of genesinvolvedin socialbehaviour(e.g.,oxytocinandvasopressin)orneuralplasticity (e.g., bdnf and npas4) [77,78]). Together, these neuronal-activity dependentmechanismschangetheneurogenomicstateofthebrain in response to perceived social stimuli. These changes in gene expressionmayoccurdifferentiallyacrossthedifferentnodesofthe neuralnetworkunderlyingtheexpressionofsocialbehaviour.Neural circuitsunderlying behaviourarecomposedbyanetworkofbrain nucleiwithreciprocalconnections betweeneachpairthatencodes information in a distributed and dynamic fashion, such that the expression ofa given behaviour isbetter reflected by theoverall profileofactivationacrossthedifferentlociinthenetworkthanbythe activity of a single node (e.g., BSBN). Different combinations of activation across nodes, and variation in the strength of the connectionsamongthem,willgenerateanalmostinfinitevariation insocialbehaviour.Therefore,thechangesinneurogenomicstates mentionedinthepreviouspointcanoccurdifferentiallyateachofthe nodesoftheBSBN,andsocialplasticityreliesbothontemporaland spatialchangesingeneregulationintheneuralnetworks.

EmA LS Mid VMH AH POA Immediate early genes Effector genes Target gene mRNA Protein FOS protein c-fos mRNA P-CREB 0 30 120 180 240 ?

TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution (a)

(b)

(e)

(c)

(d)

FigureI.Neuralandmolecularmechanismsofsocialcompetence:animalsadjusttheexpressionoftheirsocialbehaviouraccordingtoprevioussocialinteractionsand socialcontext(a);socialinformationwillbeencodedinadistributedbrainsocialbehaviournetwork(BSBN)(b);ateachnodeofthisnetwork(c)neuronswillchange theirneurogenomicstate(d),thatis,theirgeneexpressionprofileinresponsetotheperceivedsocialinformation;changesofgeneexpressionaretriggeredbythe activationofneuronalactivity-regulatedtranscriptionfactors(e.g.,pCREB)thatregulateimmediateearlygenes(e.g.,c-fos)thatcanregulatesynapticproteins(e), thereforemodulatingneuralplasticitythatunderliesbehaviouralflexibility.InthisfigurethenodesoftheBSBNarenamedafterthemammalianhomologuesofteleost brainareas(abbreviations:EmA=extendedmedialamygdala,LS=lateralseptum,Mid=centralgrayinthemidbrain,VMH=ventromedialhypothalamus,AH=anterior hypothalamus,POA=pre-opticarea).

(8)

genomicresponsestosocialinformation.ThewaveofIEG activation following a stimulus is recruiting temporally correlatedassociations inneural activityinbehaviourally significant contextsandpromotesthesloweralterationof synapticnetworks,therebyadjustingtheselectivityof long-term information storage and retrieval in neuronal net-works [60]. Thus,temporal andspatial variationin gene expression in the brain regulates the remodelling of the neuralnetworksthatunderliebehaviouralflexibility.Some IEGshavebeenshowntobeactivatedwithinminutesafter exposuretospecificsocialcuesandtovarytheiractivation withthevalence(appetitivevsaversive)andsalience(e.g., familiarityandcomplexity)ofthesocialsignal[61–63].IEG activationhasbeendocumentedinresponsetoawiderange ofsocialstimuliindifferentspeciesandsensorymodalities (e.g.,songbirds[64],Africancichlidfish[65],andTungara frogs[66]),confirmingtheroleofIEGsasneuromolecular switches for the transduction of social information into changes in brain function and behaviour. In response to IEG expression, co-regulated gene sets (‘neurogenomic states’) are then co-expressed leading to an association betweenbehaviourallydrivengeneexpressionandthe ex-pressionofsocialphenotypes(e.g.,differencesin transcrip-tome profiles between social phenotypes and contexts: hiveworkers vs foragers in honey bees [67], workers vs queens in ants [68], dominant vs subordinate fish and mammals [69,70], monogamous vs polygynous African cichlids[71],responsetosocialvssexualstimuli[72],and territorialityatdifferentlife-historystages[73]).

TheroleoftheBrain SocialBehaviourNetwork(BSBN) ingeneratingbehaviouralflexibility

Knowledgeoftheproximatemechanismsunderlyingsocial competence iscrucial tounderstandingthe costs,limits, andevolutionaryconsequences ofsocialplasticity, there-fore enabling a better understandingof the dynamicsof selection.Weproposeanintegrativeframeworkforfuture research in social competencethat integrates proximate mechanisms intothe studyofevolutionary consequences (summarised in Figure 2). According to this approach, relevant socialinformation induceschanges in neuronal activity-dependentIEGs.Giventheirpivotalrolein regu-lating gene networks, these IEGs will in turn activate massivetemporalandspatialchangesingeneexpression acrossthedifferentnodesoftheneuralnetworkunderlying socialbehaviour,theBSBN(Box2),therebyorchestrating neuralplasticityatthelevelofthenetworkandgenerating behavioural flexibility. These flexible behavioural responses will enable individuals to navigate more effi-cientlytheirsocialenvironmentatmultipledomains(e.g., defending territories, finding mates, and establishing a position in a hierarchy) thereby impacting Darwinian fitness. Since spatio-temporal changes in the neuroge-nomic states of the BSBN will give raise to an almost unlimitednumberofbehaviouralplasticity states,thisis aprimemechanismtogeneratediversityinsocial behav-iouronwhichselectionmayact,selectingthecombinations thatproduceadaptivebehaviours.However,thenumberof combinationsproducedwillbeconstrainedbyepigeneticas well as by pleiotropic and epistatic effects of the genes involvedinneuralplasticityattheBSBNlevel,imposinga

limit tobehaviouralflexibility.Thus, theoptimisation of behaviouralflexibility(i.e.,socialcompetence)willbe lim-itedbybehaviouralconsistency(i.e.,personality,see[8]), and the two should be viewed as sharing common proximatemechanismsthatshouldbetakenintoaccount whendiscussingtheirevolutionaryimplications.

Concludingremarks

Herewedrawattentiontotheevolutionaryimportanceof adaptive behavioural plasticity inthe socialdomainand propose anintegrated frameworkforits studythat com-bines investigating proximate mechanismsand ultimate consequences.Placing socialcompetenceinan evolution-aryframeworkwillfacilitateexplorationofitsevolvability andpotentialevolutionaryconsequences.Forexample,if variationbetweenindividualsexistsinsocialcompetence, individuals with a slightly better social competencewill tendtoengagemoreofteninsocialinteractions,because theycaneffectivelyreducecostsoftheseinteractionsand thereforeenjoyhighernetbenefits.Thus,theseindividuals wouldberelativelymoresocial,whichinturnwillincrease selection pressure on their social performance. By this positivefeedback,selectiononsocialcompetencemay en-hancetheevolutionofsociality.

Theintegrativeapproachproposedheretothestudyof socialcompetencehastheaddedvalueofusinginformation onthemolecularmechanismsofbehaviouralflexibilityto getaninsightintoitsevolutionarydynamics.Althoughwe describethisapproachforsocialplasticity,similar process-es can be expected to be in place for other behavioural domains,since other neuralnetworksunderlying behav-iourhavebeenidentifiedinvertebrates,andthereforethe spatio-temporal regulation of gene expression in beha-viouralbrainnetworksmaybeageneralprocessto gener-ate the neural plasticity needed to accommodate behaviouralflexibilityingeneral.

Acknowledgements

DuringthewritingofthispaperB.T.wasfundedbytheSwissNational Science Foundation (SNF, Project 31003A_133066) and R.F.O. by the PortugueseFoundation for Scienceand Technology (FCT,Grants RG-LVT-331-2352 and PTDC/PSI-PCO/118776/2010). We thank Ralph Bergmu¨ller for productive conceptual discussions on this topic that promptedthewritingofthispaper,andMiguelSimo˜esforhelpingwith thepreparationofFigureIinBox2.

References

1Frank,S.A.(2011)Naturalselection.I.Variableenvironmentsand uncertainreturnsoninvestment.J.Evol.Biol.24,2299–2309 2Aubin-Horth,N.andRenn,S.C.(2009)Genomicreactionnorms:using

integrativebiologytounderstandmolecularmechanismsofphenotypic plasticity.Mol.Ecol.18,3763–3780

3Shettleworth,S.J.(1998)Cognition,EvolutionandBehavior,Oxford UniversityPress

4Brosnan,S.F.etal.(2010)Theinterplayofcognitionandcooperation. Philos.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.365,2699–2710

5Reader,S.M.andLaland,K.N.(2002)Socialintelligence,innovation, andenhancedbrainsizeinprimates.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A.99, 4436–4441

6Piersma, T. and Drent, J. (2003) Phenotypic flexibility and the evolutionoforganismaldesign.TrendsEcol.Evol.18,228–233 7Sih,A.etal.(2004)Behavioralsyndromes:anintegrativeoverview.Q.

Rev.Biol.79,241–277

8Dingemanse, N.J.etal.(2010)Behaviouralreactionnorms:animal personalitymeetsindividualplasticity.TrendsEcol.Evol.25,81–89

Opinion

TrendsinEcologyandEvolution December2012,Vol.27,No.12

(9)

9 Burmeister,S.S.(2007)Genomicresponsestobehavioralinteractions inanAfricancichlidfish:mechanismsandevolutionaryimplications. BrainBehav.Evol.70,247–256

10 Laland,K.N.andSterelny,K.(2006)Sevenreasons(not)toneglect nicheconstruction.Evol.Dev.60,1751–1762

11 Paul,E.S.etal.(2005)Measuringemotionalprocessesinanimals:the utilityofacognitiveapproach.Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev.29,469–491 12 Waters, E. and Sroufe, L.A. (1983) Social competence as a

developmentalconstruct.Dev.Rev.3,79–97

13 White,D.J.etal.(2010)Theontogenyofsocialskills:experimental increasesinsocialcomplexityenhancereproductivesuccessinadult cowbirds.Anim.Behav.79,385–390

14 Branchi,I.etal.(2006)Earlysocialenrichmentshapessocialbehavior andnervegrowthfactorandbrain-derivedneurotrophicfactorlevelsin theadultmousebrain.Biol.Psychiatry60,690–696

15 Curley, J.P. et al. (2009) Social enrichment during postnatal development induces transgenerational effects on emotional and reproductivebehaviorinmice.Front.Behav.Neurosci.3,25 16 Arnold,C.andTaborsky,B.(2010)Socialexperienceinearlyontogeny

haslastingeffectsonsocialskillsincooperativelybreedingcichlids. Anim.Behav.79,621–630

17 Taborsky,B.etal.(2012)Theearlysocialenvironmentaffectssocial competenceinacooperativebreeder.Anim.Behav.83,1067–1074 18Dodge,K.(1985)Facetsofsocialinteractionandtheassessmentofsocial

competenceinchildren.InChildren’sPeerRelations:IssuesinAssessment andIntervention(Schneider,B.etal.,eds), pp.3–22,Springer

19 Rose-Krasnor,L.(1997)Thenatureofsocialcompetence:Atheoretical review.Soc.Dev.6,111–135

20 Doutrelant,C.etal.(2001)Theeffectofanaudienceonintrasexual communicationinmaleSiamesefightingfish,Bettasplendens.Behav. Ecol.12,283–286

21 Pinto,A.etal.(2011)CleanerwrassesLabroidesdimidiatusaremore cooperativeinthepresenceofanaudience.Curr.Biol.21,1140–1144 22 Oliveira,R.F.etal.(1998)Knowthineenemy:fighting fishgather informationfromobservingconspecificinteractions.Proc.R.Soc.Lond. B:Biol.Sci.265,1045–1049

23 Earley, R.L. (2010) Social eavesdropping and the evolution of conditional cooperation and cheating strategies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.365,2675–2686

24 Temeles,E.J.(1994)Theroleofneighboursinterritorialsystems:when arethey‘dearenemies’?Anim.Behav.47,339–350

25 Mu¨ller,C.A.andManser,M.B.(2007)‘Nastyneighbours’ratherthan ‘dearenemies’inasocialcarnivore.Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.274, 959–965

26 Rutte,C.etal.(2006)Whatsetstheoddsofwinningandlosing?Trends Ecol.Evol.21,16–21

27 Hsu,Y.etal.(2006)Modulationofaggressivebehaviourbyfighting experience:mechanismsandcontestoutcomes.Biol.Rev.81,33–74 28 Rutte,C.andTaborsky,M.(2007)Generalizedreciprocityinrats.PLoS

Biol.5,e196

29 Lehner,S.R.etal.(2011)Ratsbenefitfromwinnerandlosereffects. Ethology117,949–960

30 Bshary,R.andGrutter,A.S.(2002)Asymmetriccheatingopportunities andpartnercontrolinacleanerfishmutualism.Anim.Behav.63,547– 555

31 Neisser, U.et al. (1996)Intelligence: knowns and unknowns. Am. Psychol.51,77

32 Lefebvre,L.etal.(2004)Brains,innovationsandevolutioninbirdsand primates.BrainBehav.Evol.63,233–246

33 Byrne,R.W.andBates,L.A.(2007)Sociality,evolutionandcognition. Curr.Biol.17,R714–R723

34 Reader,S.M.etal.(2011)Theevolutionofprimategeneralandcultural intelligence.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.B366,1017–1027

35 Heyes,C.(2012)What’ssocialaboutsociallearning?J.Comp.Psychol. 126,193–202

36 Levy,F.etal.(2003)Completematernaldeprivationaffectssocial,but notspatial,learninginadultrats.Dev.Psychobiol.43,177–191 37 D’Andrea, I. et al. (2007) Communal nesting, an early social

enrichment,affectssocialcompetencesbutnotlearningandmemory abilitiesatadulthood.Behav.BrainRes.183,60–66

38 Branchi, I. (2009) The mouse communal nest: investigating the epigeneticinfluences oftheearly socialenvironmenton brainand behaviordevelopment.Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev.33,551–559

39 Moretz,J.A.etal.(2007)Behavioralsyndromesandtheevolutionof correlatedbehaviorinzebrafish.Behav.Ecol.18,556–562

40 Baron-Cohen,S.etal.(2001)The‘‘Readingthemindintheeyes’’test revisedversion:Astudywithnormaladults,andadultswithAsperger syndromeorhigh-functioningautism.J.ChildPsychol.Psychiatry42, 241–251

41 van der Maas, H.L. et al. (2006) A dynamical model of general intelligence: the positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychol.Rev.113,842–861

42 Arnold, S.J. (1983) Sexual selection: the interface of theory and empiricism. In Mate Choice (Bateson, P., ed.), pp. 67–107, CambridgeUniversityPress

43 Irschick, D.J. et al. (2008) How does selection operate on whole-organism functional performance capacities? A review and synthesis.Evol.Ecol.Res.10,177–196

44 McGuire,S.etal.(1999)Perceivedcompetenceandself-worthduring adolescence:alongitudinalbehavioral geneticstudy.ChildDev.70, 1283–1296

45 Kuo,P.H.etal.(2004)Atwinstudyofcompetenceandbehavioral/ emotionalproblemsamongadolescentsinTaiwan.Behav.Genet.34, 63–74

46 Margulis,S.W.etal.(2005)Effectsofearlyexperienceonsubsequent parental behaviour and reproductive success in oldfield mice, Peromyscuspolionotus.Anim.Behav.69,627–634

47 Auld,J.R.etal.(2010)Re-evaluatingthecostsandlimitsofadaptive phenotypicplasticity.Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.277,503–511 48 Dunbar, R.I.M.andShultz,S.(2007)Evolutioninthesocialbrain.

Science317,1344–1347

49 Dunbar,R.I.M.(1998)Thesocialbrainhypothesis.Evol.Anthropol.6, 178–190

50 Chittka,L.andNiven,J.(2009)Arebiggerbrainsbetter?Curr.Biol. 19,R995–R1008

51 Dukas,R.(1999)Costsofmemory:ideasandpredictions.J.Theor.Biol. 197,41–50

52 Burns,J.G.etal.(2011)Costsofmemory:lessonsfrom‘mini’brains. Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.278,923–929

53 Grantner,A.andTaborsky,M.(1998)Themetabolicratesassociated withresting,andwiththeperformanceofagonistic,submissiveand diggingbehavioursinthecichlidfishNeolamprologuspulcher(Pisces: Cichlidae).J.Comp.Physiol.B168,427–433

54 Chittka,L.etal.(2009)Speed-accuracytradeoffsinanimaldecision making.TrendsEcol.Evol.24,400–407

55 Mery,F.andKawecki,T.J.(2003)Afitnesscostoflearningabilityin Drosophilamelanogaster.Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.270,2465–2469 56 Kolss, M.and Kawecki, T.J.(2008) Reduced learning ability as a consequence of evolutionary adaptation to nutritional stress in Drosophilamelanogaster.Ecol.Entomol.33,583–588

57 DeWitt, T.J.etal. (1998)Costsandlimitsof phenotypicplasticity. TrendsEcol.Evol.13,77–81

58 Pigliucci,M.(2005)Evolutionofphenotypicplasticity:wherearewe goingnow?TrendsEcol.Evol.20,481–486

59 Colvis,C.M.etal.(2005)Epigeneticmechanismsandgenenetworksin thenervoussystem.J.Neurosci.25,10379–10389

60 Clayton,D.F.(2000)Thegenomicactionpotential.Neurobiol.Learn. Mem.74,185–216

61 Jarvis,E.D.etal.(1998)Forwhomthebirdsings:context-dependent geneexpression.Neuron21,775–788

62 Mello,C.V.etal.(1992)Songpresentationinducesgeneexpressionin thesongbirdforebrain.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A.89,6818 63 Dong,S.andClayton,D.F.(2008)Partialdissociationofmolecularand

behavioralmeasuresofsonghabituationinadultzebrafinches.Genes BrainBehav.7,802–809

64 Mello,C.V.andJarvis,E.D.(2008)Behavior-dependentexpressionof inducible genesinvocal learningbirds.In Neuroscienceof BirdSong (Zeigler,H.P.andMarler,P.,eds),pp.381–397,CambridgeUniversity Press

65 Burmeister,S.S.etal.(2005)Rapidbehavioralandgenomicresponses tosocialopportunity.PLoSBiol.3,e363

66 Burmeister,S.S.etal.(2008)Acousticmodulationofimmediateearly gene expressioninthe auditorymidbrain offemale tungara frogs. BrainRes.1190,105–114

67 Whitfield, C.W.et al.(2003) Geneexpressionprofilesin thebrain predictbehaviorinindividualhoneybees.Science302,296–299

(10)

68 Gra¨ff,J.etal.(2007)Differentialgeneexpressionbetweenadultqueens andworkersintheantLasiusniger.Mol.Ecol.16,675–683 69 Kroes, R.A. et al. (2006) Modeling depression: social

dominance-submissiongeneexpressionpatternsinratneocortex. Neuroscience 137,37–49

70 Renn,S.C.etal.(2008)Fishandchips:functionalgenomicsofsocial plasticityinanAfricancichlidfish.J.Exp.Biol.211,3041–3056 71 Machado,H.E.etal.(2009)Interspecificprofilingofgeneexpression

informedbycomparativegenomichybridization:Areviewandanovel approachinAfricancichlidfishes.Integr.Comp.Biol.49,644–659 72 Cummings,M.E.etal.(2008)Sexualandsocialstimulielicitrapidand

contrastinggenomicresponses.Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B:Biol.Sci.275, 393–402

73 Mukai,M.etal.(2009)Seasonaldifferencesofgeneexpressionprofiles in song sparrow(Melospiza melodia) hypothalamus in relation to territorialaggression.PLoSONE4,e8182

74 Stiver,K.etal.(2004)Dispersalpatternsandstatuschangeina co-operativelybreedingcichlidNeolamprologuspulcher: evidencefrom microsatelliteanalysesandbehaviouralobservations.J.FishBiol.65, 91–105

75 Goodson, J.L. (2005) The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionarythemesandvariations.Horm.Behav.48,11–22

76 Wolf,C.andLinden,D.E.J.(2012)Biologicalpathwaystoadaptability – interactions between genome, epigenome, nervous system and environmentforadaptivebehavior.GenesBrainBehav.11,3–28 77 Champagne, F.A. and Curley, J.P. (2005) How social experiences

influencethebrain.Curr.Opin.Neurobiol.15,704–709

78 Curley,J.P.etal.(2011)Socialinfluencesonneurobiologyandbehavior: epigeneticeffectsduringdevelopment.Psychoneuroendocrino36,352– 371

79 Adkins-Regan,E.andKrakauer,A.(2000)Removalofadultmalesfrom therearingenvironmentincreasespreferenceforsame-sexpartnersin thezebrafinch.Anim.Behav.60,47–53

80 Branchi,I.etal.(2009)Birthspacinginthemousecommunalnest shapesadultemotionalandsocialbehavior.Physiol.Behav.96,532– 539

81 Musi, B. et al. (1993) Influence of litter gender composition on subsequentmaternal behaviourand maternalaggressioninfemale housemice.Ethology95,43–53

82 Sharpe,R.(1975)Theinfluenceofthesexoflitter-matesonsubsequent maternalbehaviourinRattusnorvegicus.Anim.Behav.23,551–559 83 Bastian,M.L.etal.(2003)Long-termeffectsofinfantrearingcondition

ontheacquisition ofdominancerank injuvenileand adultrhesus macaques(Macacamulatta).Dev.Psychobiol.42,44–51

Opinion

TrendsinEcologyandEvolution December2012,Vol.27,No.12

Imagem

Figure 1. Social competence is hypothesised to be an ability underlying all social behaviour that is contingent on social information and thus to affect behaviour in many or all social contexts
Table I. Three behavioural tests to assess the differences in social competence between N
Table 1. Summary of studies in which (i) the social experience during ontogeny was manipulated, (ii) challenge tests were conducted after the manipulations to test for induced differences in social performance, and (iii) effects of differential social beha
Figure 2. Path diagram showing the proposed relationship between the social environment (E1–En), immediate early genes (IEG1 and IEG2), neurogenomic states of relevant neural circuits (N1–Nk), that will determine the neural and behavioural plasticity that
+2

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Purpose: to characterize the behavioral problems and social competence of individuals with del22q11.2 syndrome and compare them with typically developing individuals, according

Neste âmbito, e enquanto farmacêutico estagiário, foi importante perceber quais as responsabilidades inerentes a este serviço, sendo que estas englobam a vertente

We compute the company market risk premium (MRP) assuming the sum of the US MRP and the average between the countries risk premium (CRP) of the main market share:

Contudo, no que diz respeito ao caso de prolação do despacho saneador- sentença, com vista ao termo ao processo, a dispensa da audiência prévia revelou ser, a meu ver, uma

Two dimensions make the representation social: the context of a subject’s social interaction or confrontation with a social stimulus; and the social subject, which intervenes in the

O artigo surge através de uma reflexão desenvolvida no curso de mestrado em Letras e Ciências Humanas da Unigranrio, em virtude das discussões sobre o tema

The article identifies the mechanisms of social legitimation and artistic influence, as well as the cultural and social implications of social networks in the contemporary

These hypotheses are: (1) school and family climate are directly and inversely related with school violence, while integration in the community does not have a direct relationship