• Nenhum resultado encontrado

CoDAS vol.25 número1 en v25n1a13

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2018

Share "CoDAS vol.25 número1 en v25n1a13"

Copied!
6
0
0

Texto

(1)

Artigo Original

Liliane Perroud Miilher1 Fernanda Dreux Miranda Fernandes1

Descritores

Linguagem Transtorno autístico Estudos de linguagem Comunicação Fonoaudiologia

Keywords

Language Autistic disorder Language arts Communication Speech, language and hearing sciences

Correspondence address: Liliane Perroud Miilher

R. Cipotânea, 51, Cidade Universitária, São Paulo (SP), Brasil, CEP: 05360-000. E-mail: li.miilher@usp.br

Received: 5/21/2012

Accepted: 10/15/2012

Study conducted at the Department of Physical Therapy, Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, School of Medicine, Universidade de São Paulo – USP – São Paulo (SP), Brazil.

(1) Department of Physical Therapy, Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, School of Medicine, Uni-versidade de São Paulo – USP – São Paulo (SP), Brazil.

Conflict of interests: None

analysis in autism spectrum

Considerando a responsividade: uma proposta de análise

pragmática no espectro do autismo

ABSTRACT

Purpose: To compare the pragmatic profile referring to the communicative initiatives and the bi-dimensional profile involving the aspects of initiative and responsivity. It also aimed to analyze the most common types of responses presented by the studied individuals. Methods: Thirty recorded samples of interaction between speech-language therapist and children with autism spectrum disorders (mean age: 9 years and 6 months) ses-sions were analyzed. The samples were transcribed and data analyzed about number of communicative acts, occupation of the communicative space, use of communicative means (verbal, vocal and gestural) and total number of participations (initiatives and responses). The responses were qualified as “non-answer”, “adequate answer”, “inadequate answer” and “pragmatically inappropriate answer”. Results: Significant differences in the comparison of the numbers of initiatives and total participations and of occupation of communicative space and total number of communicative acts. There was also a significant difference in the number of “adequate answers”. Conclusion: Results show the need to consider the bi-dimensional communicative profile and qualify the answers in order to determine the child’s communication abilities.

RESUMO

(2)

INTRODUCTION

Autism was described more than 60 years ago and is now considered a pervasive developmental disorder that involves cognitive, social and linguistic areas. Although the 3 areas are always affected to some extent, there are important variations from one individual to the other. Language may be impaired on several levels, specially on pragmatic aspects(1,2), and it is

considered an important prognostic element(3,4).

Pragmatic competence includes knowledge about language structure as well as about the rules that govern social behavior and environmental reality(5,6). It can also be considered a system

to the interpretation of world phenomena and human actions(7).

In this perspective there is a close association with the notion of social cognition and the ability to understand nonverbal communication signs and make complex inferences about other people’s behavior, thinking and beliefs(8). There are different

ways to assess the pragmatic aspects(9-11), from the analysis of

speech acts to the conversational macrostructure description(12-17).

The communicative act is the main unit of pragmatic analy-sis when the focus is placed on the monologue, that is, when each communicative turn is observed, regardless of the general dialog organization(18). They may be studied regarding their

lan-guage structure, the intentions they convey (this is the general focus of analysis) and their effects on the listener. However, the analyzed acts are necessarily those that fit on a certain idea of assertivity. Especially in the case of autism spectrum disorders, the importance of initiating communicative exchanges refers to the fact that it shows the intention of communicating with the other person using all available resources. Such intention is the overlapping point of desiring to communicate, wanting to be understood and understanding the necessary social mechanisms to achieve the task. Therefore these initiatives are the privileged meeting point between one individual and the other.

However, communicative exchanges are two-fold. On one side there is the initiative and on the other the responsivity(19).

The exchange will only be effective if the participants share a common interactional nucleus(20). During a dialog it is effective

through an adjust between speech and silent segments and non--verbal elements(21,22). A statement with no answer is considered

a communicative failure by the initiator and therefore most answers have an important value in maintaining the communi-cative flow. Some authors(18,21) have been studding the

responsi-vity issue in children with pragmatic language disorders. The authors proposed four types of adjust between the precedent speech and the answer. This way, they aimed to understand the harmony degree determined between interlocutors. Answering means understanding the precedent speech, being interested in continuing to talk, wanting to provide new information and guarantee turn exchanges. This way, answering is – as well as the initiatives – another privileged meeting point between one individual and the other.

Aiming to improve the understanding about the communi-cation process of children of the autism spectrum, this study had the purpose to compare the functional communicative profile of the communicative initiatives with a bi-dimensional profile that includes initiatives and answers. Besides, the most

common types of answers presented by the studied individuals were analyzed.

METHODS

The research was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the School of Medicine of Universidade de São Paulo under protocol number 091/10, and an adult responsible by each child signed the approved consent form.

Research corpus was comprised by three samples of 10 different patients (total of 30 samples). All subjects were diagnosed by psychiatrists according to the criteria proposed by the DSM-IV(23) and the IDC-10(24). The classification

re-gistered in the medial reports was maintained, as displayed in Chart 1. All subjects were assessed and attended speech--language therapy on a specialized service (LIF-DEA) at the Communication Sciences and Disorders Program of School of Medicine – Universidade de São Paulo during the period of this study. None of the subjects presented other diseases or hearing complaint. The inclusion criteria was the spontaneous use of oral language.

Among the ten subjects, just one was a girl (Chart 1). All the subjects interacted with female therapists that were gradu-ate students of a training practice program in speech-language therapy with childhood psychiatric disorders.

The analysis used filmed samples of three therapeutic situations in which the therapists interacted with the subjects following specific guidelines regarding the initial presentation of the material. All the recording sessions were conducted du-ring the second semesters of 2009 and 2010 because at these periods the dyad’s members were already acquainted with each other for at least six months. This procedure was adopted in all the recordings aiming to diminish the impact of the interference of the familiarity among the interlocutors(25).

The filmed samples were obtained specifically to the present

Chart 1. Research subjects and video sampling order

Subject Gender Age Therapist Diagnosis

1 Male 11y5m A Autism

2 Male 6y4m B High functioning autism

3 Male 6y2m B Asperger

syndrome

4 Male 5y6m C Asperger

syndrome 5 Male 13y2m D Autism spectrum 6 Male 6y0m B High functioning

autism 7 Male 16y8m E Autism spectrum 8 Female 13y1m E Atypical autism 9 Male 7y5m A Autism spectrum 10 Male 11y0m B Autism spectrum

Mean 9y6m

(3)

study and the procedures followed the general orientation of open proposals where it was allowed (and encouraged) that each dyad created new play situations. The proposal referred to three play situations (a birthday party; create a history based on a play-book and teaching to assembly a toy based on the toy’s manual) with different demands (respectively: a social demand, a linguistic demand and a cognitive demand).

The Functional Communicative Profile(26) was used to

assess the communicative acts. Besides the communicative functions, the communicative means used in each act were also analyzed: verbal mean (emissions with at least 75% of the correct phonemes); vocal mean (emissions with less than 75% of the correct phonemes) and gestural mean (including body and face movements).

The filmed sample was transcribed and analyzed according to the incidence of each communicative function and mean, proportion of occupation of the communicative space, number of communicative acts expressed per minute and proportion of communicative acts expressing interpersonal communicative functions.

The Functional Communicative Profile considers the com-municative initiatives of both interlocutors. But conversational aspects include not just the initiatives but also how much one of the interlocutors is responsive to the other’s initiatives.

Therefore, to assess the responsivity the following catego-ries of answers were considered(21):

- Non-answer (NA): when there was a clear opportunity for the subject to answer but it wasn’t used;

- Adequate answer (AA): when the answer was adjusted to the prior question or demand;

- Inadequate answer (IA): when the answer provided wasn’t satisfactory due to linguistic limitations, misunderstandings or general issues regarding the comprehension of reality; - Pragmatically inappropriate answer (PI): when there wasn’t

a satisfactory adjustment of the answer’s social and com-municative context to the demand.

The non-answer (NA) category posed some troubles to the analysis. Unlikely the other types of answers this one is defined by the absence and not by the presence of a certain characteris-tic. Furthermore, to determine the absence of answer implies in the certainty that the opportunity to answer was guaranteed. Even with the turn exchanging the interlocutor may opt not to answer or to ignore the questioning or even to initiate another topic, organizing ideas. Considering these difficulties – that are frequent in conversational contexts – the NA was strictly classified just when there was a communicative breakdown determined by the absence of the expected answer. Therefore, even situations where one interlocutor asked something and the other didn’t answer were not always classified as NA. The main criterion to the classification of NA was the identification of a communicative breakdown due to the lack of answer.

To include the answers the Functional Communicative Profile protocol was completed with the following items (Appendix 1):

- Types of answers;

- Number of participations (including initiatives and an-swers);

- Communicative space (of initiatives and general – initiatives and answers);

- Communicative means (used in the initiatives and in general - initiatives and answers).

The analysis of the Functional Communicative Profile deals with the communication initiatives and the responsivity items refer to the answers. An item about “total of participations” was created, including the initiatives as well as the answers.

Therefore, after the original protocol and the information about responsivity were completed, the software that was spe-cially developed to this research calculated the following items referring to the participation: general use of communicative means; proportion of initiatives and answers; occupation of the communicative space. This last item considered also the total participation of the adult, ensuring the fidelity of data.

The data were compared within variables through the Analysis of Variance – Anova. The significance level adopted was 0.05 (5%). When there was statistical significance the t--Student test was used to analyze the pair of variables where the difference was identified.

RESULTS

Table 1 presents the values of average and variance of participations, initiatives and answers of the subjects during the interaction.

The comparison of the number of initiatives and the total number of participations (i.e., the sum of the initiatives and answers) revealed statistical difference. This result shows the difference of considering just the assertive aspects of communi-cation without observing the double communicative movement of initiatives and answers.

Table 2 shows the use of communicative means. The values between brackets refer to the variance of data. The comparison of the three communicative means in each category of analysis (initiatives, answers and total of participations) has shown that there was significant difference in all of them (p>0.001). In the comparison of the use of each communicative mean in initia-tives, answers and total of participations the only difference was related to the vocal mean. The pairwise comparison has shown that the three pairs of variables were different among each other (p>0.001 in all cases).

The average values of occupation of the communicative space, as well as the significance values determined in the comparison of data are presented in Table 3. That ratio of com-municative space considering all of the child’s participation was

Table 1. Comparison between initiatives, answers and the total par-ticipations

Statistics Participations Initiatives Answers

Mean 45.80 34.37 11.43

Variance 97.75 100.65 21.98

p-value >0.001*

(4)

different of the traditional ratio (considering just the initiatives). The comparison between the types of answers (NA, AA, IA and PI) is synthesized in Table 4. The pairwise comparison has shown that AA had a statistically different behavior than the other cathegories.

DISCUSSION

The results indicate that there is a difference between an analysis that considers all communicative participations or just the initiatives. The social engagement difficulties there are fre-quent in the autism spectrum disorders are mainly responsible by the smaller number of communicative initiatives and other pragmatic impairments(1,2). Certainly initiating an interaction

and maintaining it on the expected form (i.e., exchanging turns and respecting the topic contingency) is a challenge to all speakers because it requires the use of linguistic, social and cognitive resources. They are necessary because there is the understanding that interactional exchanges are effective just when there is reciprocity.

A reciprocal interaction involves a tacit agreement between participants that all will contribute to the successful commu-nication. There are undeclared (nevertheless, shared) common purposes among interlocutors, what turns interaction into a complex social experience(20). It implies that interactions are

cooperative and inter-subjective because the speakers must un-derstand what is said as well as express their own unun-derstanding about it(27). This understanding and revealing movement is also

experimented during interaction through the turn-taking and the conditional sequence of the interventions. The sequence is conditioned by what was said/done before and that will, to some extent, determine what will be said/done afterwards. To participate on an interaction, as a conversation for example, the person needs to understand and answer appropriately(19) and to

do it not just the content of the communication is important, but also the form aspects of the contribution(20).

The pragmatic analyses of the communicative functions, by their nature, assess the initiative movements. Considering the reverse movement, that is, the responsivity allows the understanding about how participative a certain individual is during a determined situation. This way, adding the information about responsivity to those regarding the initiatives provides an overview about general interaction structures. This perspective revealed a significant difference, when the total of interventions by the children and the occupation of the communicative space were considered (Tables 1 and 3, respectively). When just the initiatives are considered there is a tendency of not attributing to the child any responsibility for a collaborative exchange because their ability to adjust to requests and demands are not considered. The bi-dimensional analysis – considering initiati-ves and answers - attributes the appropriate value to the double role on communication, i.e., as speaker and as listener, as shown in turn-taking and sequencing rules. Research that assess the occupation of the communicative space(25,28,29) show that the

children often have a smaller occupation ratio. However, the communicative “space” is a virtual concept that occurs during the communicative exchange and can only be understood with reference to the interactive stile. Some of the literature men-tioned(28) also suggest that the therapists use communicative

functions of requests that imply on an answer by the other. If the therapists make more requests, it is desirable that the patients respond to the requests, although these answers still have not been considered in the functional analysis.

The use of communicative means indicated that only the vocal mean had a significant difference between answers, initia-tives and the total of participations (Table 2). It can be related to the smaller use of validating resources by children with autism spectrum disorders, since many of them are vocal markers(30).

Besides that, an answer should offer new information, ratify or rectify a prior statement and verbal and gestural resources are more appropriate to these functions.

The surprising result was the significant difference betwe-en AA and the other types of answers, which was differbetwe-ent from prior studies(13,21). It can be related to the interactive

context, where the information about objects and events

Table 2. Average use of communicative means in initiatives, answers and total of participations

Statistics Verbal mean (variance) Vocal mean (variance) Gestual mean (variance) p-value

Initiatives 66.9 (452.16) 7.6 (74.4) 45.53 (259.84) >0.001*

Answers 64.6 (866.24) 0.2 (1.2) 40.79 (898.74) >0.001*

Participations 66.75 (478.02) 5.7(44.25) 42.51 (322.8) >0.001*

p-value 0.92 >0.001 0.89

* Significant value s (p≤0.05) – ANOVA

Table 3. Occupation of the communicative space referring to the initia-tives and to the total participations: average and significance

Statistics Initiatives Participations p-value

Mean 39.1 45.77

>0.001* Variance 42.57 23.15

* Significant value (p≤0.05) – Student ‘s t test

Table 4. Average and variance values for each type of answer in the three assessment situations

Type of answer Mean Variance p-value Non answer (NA) 1.66 31.26

>0.001* Adequate answer (AA) 95.96 59.41

Inadequate answer (IA) 0.86 4.67 Pragmatically inappropriate

answer (PI) 1.63 24.37

(5)

where limited to the immediate context, familiar to adults and children. That is, during an interaction determined by an specific proposal the number of variables is smaller, the linguistic structures refer to a more circumscribed environ-ment(6) that is, therefore, less challenging. Or even because

as the proposal’s demand don’t rely on the emotional factor, the participants didn’t have difficulties to answer appropria-tely(21). The ability to discriminate (and not only identify)

the adjustment’s degree between the adult’s demand and the child’s answer provides information about the comprehension level and the kind of difficulties presented by the subjects(18),

allowing more effective intervention proposals.

The limited corpus is one of the main limitations of this research, but it is usual in studies involving detailed microa-nalysis(19). Other research may assess if a larger sample would

produce similar results. Other limitation refers to the kind of interaction that was analyzed. This research used video samples of interaction with specific themes. Other types of interaction (for example, social exchanges in home environment, school or free exchanges) may present different results than the ones found in this research.

CONCLUSION

Analyzing the responsivity and considering it on the com-prehension of the communicative profile of children of the autism spectrum provide information about the interactions’ global structures and broader communicative abilities. This information has an undeniable value to the language analysis but may be especially important to the therapeutic planning. Understanding the how much a child is assertive and responsive provides a notion about the kind of communicator the child is and what steps should be taken to achieve better communica-tion effectivity.

* LPM was responsible for data collection and analysis, and general elaboration of the manuscript. FDMF contributed with the general elaboration of the manuscript and conclusion of the study.

REFERENCES

1. Whitehouse AJO, Maybery MT, Durkin K. Evidence against poor semantic encoding in individuals with autism. Autism. 2007 May;11(3):241-54.

2. Harris GJ, Chabris CF, Clark J, Urban T, Aharon I, Steele S, et al. Brain activation during semantic processing in autism spectrum disorders via functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Cogn. 2006 Jun;61(1):54-68.

3. Folstein SE, Rosen-Shedley B. Genetics of autism: complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nat Rev Genet. 2001 Dec;2(12):943-55. 4. Volkmar FR, Pauls D. Autism. Lancet. 2003 Oct 4;362(9390):1133-41. 5. Thompson L. The development of pragmatic competence: past findings

and future directions for research. Current Issues in Language and Society. 1996;3(1):3-21.

6. Wittek A, Tomasello M. Young children’s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2005 Oct;26(4):541-58.

7. Carston R. Linguistic meaning, communicated meaning and cognitive pragmatics. Mind &Language. 2002 Feb-Apr;17(1-2):127-48.

8. Silliman ER, Diehl SF, Bahr RH, Hnath-Chisolm T, Zenko CB, Friedman SA. A new look at performance on theory-of-mind tasks by adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2003 Jul;34:236-52.

9. Halliday MAK. Language as social semiotic: the social interpretation of language and meaning. Maryland: University Park Press, 1978. 10. Adams C. Practitioner review: the assessment of language pragmatics. J

Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002 Nov;43(8):973-87.

11. Hage SRV, Resegue MM, Viveiros DCS, Pacheco EF. Análise do perfil das habilidades pragmáticas em crianças pequenas normais. Pro Fono. 2007 Jan-Abr;19(1):49-58.

12. Hale CM, Tager-Flusberg H. Brief report: the relationship between discourse deficits and autism symptomatology. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005 Aug;35(4):519-24.

13. Eigsti IM, Bennetto L, Dadlani MB. Beyond pragmatics: morphosyntactic development in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007Jul;37(6):1007-23.

14. de Villiers J, Fine J, Ginsberg G, Vaccarella L, Szatmari P. Brief report: a scale for rating conversational impairment in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007 Aug;37(7):1375-80.

15. Colle L, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, van der Lely HK. Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008 Jan;38(1):28-40.

16. Goldman S. Brief report: narratives of personal events in children with autism and developmental language disorders: unshared memories. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008 Nov;38(10):1982-8.

17. Paul R, OrlovskiM S, Marcinko HC, Volkmar F. Conversation behaviors in youth with higt-functioning ASD and asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009 Jan;39(1):115-25.

18. Adams C, Lloyd J. Elicited and spontaneous communicative functions and stability of conversational measures with children who have pragmatic language impairments. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2005 Jul;40(3):333-47.

19. Bishop DVM, Chan J, Adams C, Hartley J, Weir F. Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12(2):177-99.

20. Branigan HP, Pickering MJ, Pearson J, McLean JF. Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics. 2010 Sep;42(9):2355-68.

21. Adams C, Green J, Gilchrist A, Cox A. Conversational behavior of children with Asperger syndrome and conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002 Jul;43(5):679-90.

22. Geils C, Knoetze J. Conversations with Barney: a conversation analysis of interactions with a child with autism. South African Journal of Psychology. 2008;38(1):200-24.

23. American Psychiatric Association. Manual de diagnóstico e estatística de transtornos mentais – DSM-IV. 4a ed. Porto Alegre: ArtesMédicas; 1995. 24. Organização Mundial da Saúde. CID-10: classificação de transtornos

mentais e de comportamento a CID-10: critérios diagnósticos para pesquisa. 10a ed.Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas; 1993.

25. Moreira CR, Fernandes FDM. Avaliação da comunicação no espectro autístico: interferência da familiaridade no desempenho de linguagem. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15(3):5.

26. Fernandes FDM. Pragmática. In: Andrade CRF, Befi-Lopes DM, Fernandes FDM, Wertzner HF. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática. 2a ed. Carapicuíba: Pró-Fono; 2004. p.83-97.

27. Garcez PM. A perspeciva da análise da conversa etnometodológica sobre o uso da linguagem em interação social. In: Loder LL, Jung NM (org). Fala-em-interação social: uma introdução à análise da conversa etnometodológica. Campinas: Mercado das Letras; 2008. p.17-38. 28. Miilher LP, Fernandes FDM. Análise das funções comunicativas

expressas por terapeutas e pacientes do espectro autístico. Pro Fono. 2006;18(3):239-48.

(6)

Appendix 1. Functional Communicative Profile – Enlarged

Name: Diagnosis: Examiner:

DoB: Age: Date: DVD #:

Time _____ minutes

Communicative acts A _____ _____% Participations # functions

C _____ _____% Initiatives _____ _____% Interactive functions _____ TOTAL _____ _____% Answers _____ _____% Interactive acts _____ Communicative acts per

minute_____

Function Mean N % Function Mean N % Function Mean N % ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

PO VO __ __ PS VO __ __ PI VO __ __

____% G __ __ ____% G __ __ _____% G __ __

______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

RO VO __ __ C VO __ __ N VO __ __

____% G __ __ ____% G __ __ _____% G __ __ Answers Mean N % ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

EX VO __ __ NF VO __ __ XP VO __ __ NR VO __ __

____% G __ __ ____% G __ __ _____% G __ __ ______% G __ __

______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

EP VO __ __ PA VO __ __ PC VO __ __ RA VO __ __

____% G __ __ ____% G __ __ _____% G __ __ ______% G __ __

______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

PR VO __ __ E VO __ __ AR VO __ __ RI VO __ __

____% G __ __ ____% G __ __ _____% G __ __ ______% G __ __

______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

PE VO __ __ JC VO __ __ J VO __ __ RPInap VO __ __

____% G __ __ ____% G __ __ _____% G __ __ ______% G __ __

______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __ ______ VE __ __

NA VO __ __ RE VO __ __ TOTAL VO __ __ Tot.

Answers

VO __ __

Imagem

Table 1 presents the values of average and variance of  participations, initiatives and answers of the subjects during  the interaction.
Table 2. Average use of communicative means in initiatives, answers and total of participations

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Este artigo discute o filme Voar é com os pássaros (1971) do diretor norte-americano Robert Altman fazendo uma reflexão sobre as confluências entre as inovações da geração de

Despercebido: não visto, não notado, não observado, ignorado.. Não me passou despercebido

Este modelo permite avaliar a empresa como um todo e não apenas o seu valor para o acionista, considerando todos os cash flows gerados pela empresa e procedendo ao seu desconto,

apresentamos um possível processo de tratamento do rio Camarajipe dividido em três partes, onde a primeira teria início em laboratório, com testes para analisar o desempenho

Na hepatite B, as enzimas hepáticas têm valores menores tanto para quem toma quanto para os que não tomam café comparados ao vírus C, porém os dados foram estatisticamente

The fourth generation of sinkholes is connected with the older Đulin ponor-Medvedica cave system and collects the water which appears deeper in the cave as permanent

The irregular pisoids from Perlova cave have rough outer surface, no nuclei, subtle and irregular lamination and no corrosional surfaces in their internal structure (Figure

Because the artistic education of people is, in many cases, better developed than their grasp of science, it would appear more useful to employ a metaphorical description of the