• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Efeitos do canto de anúncio e do tamanho corporal no espaçamento entre machos em agregações de Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Share "Efeitos do canto de anúncio e do tamanho corporal no espaçamento entre machos em agregações de Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae)"

Copied!
49
0
0

Texto

(1)

Verônica Thiemi Tsutae de Sousa

Efeitos do canto de anúncio e do tamanho corporal no

espaçamento entre machos em agregações de

Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae)

Dissertação apresentada para obtenção do título de Mestre em Biologia Animal, área de Ecologia e Comportamento Animal, junto ao Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biologia Animal do Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas da Universidade Estadual Paulista “Júlio de Mesquita Filho”, Campus de São José do Rio Preto.

Orientador: Prof. Dr. Christopher Gordon Murphy

Co-orientadora: Profª. Drª. Denise de Cerqueira Rossa-Feres

(2)

Sousa, Verônica Thiemi Tsutae de.

Efeitos do canto de anúncio e do tamanho corporal no espaçamento entre machos em agregações de Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae)/ Verônica Thiemi Tsutae de Sousa. - São José do Rio Preto: [s.n.], 2012.

49 f.: 8 il.; 30 cm.

Orientador: Christopher Gordon Murphy

Co-orientadora: Denise de Cerqueira Rossa Feres

Dissertação (Mestrado) - Universidade Estadual Paulista, Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas

1. Assunto. 2. Assunto. 3. Assunto. I. Murphy, Christopher Gordon. II. Rossa-Feres, Denise de Cerqueira. III. Universidade Estadual Paulista, Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas. IV. Efeitos do canto de anúncio e do tamanho corporalno espaçamento entre machos em agregações de Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae).

(3)

-Efeitos do canto de anúncio e do tamanho corporal no

espaçamento entre machos em agregações de

Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae)

Dissertação apresentada para obtenção do título de Mestre em Biologia Animal, área de Ecologia e Comportamento Animal, junto ao Programa de Pós-Graduação em Biologia Animal do Instituto de Biociências, Letras e Ciências Exatas da Universidade

Estadual Paulista “Júlio de Mesquita Filho”, Campus de São José do Rio

Preto.

BANCA EXAMINADORA

Profª. Drª. Denise de Cerqueira Rossa-Feres

Professor Assistente Doutor UNESP – São José do Rio Preto Co-orientadora

Profª. Drª. Cynthia Peralta de Almeida Prado

Professor Assistente Doutor UNESP – Jaboticabal

Prof. Dr. Itamar Alves Martins Professor Assistente Doutor Universidade de Taubaté

(4)
(5)

-AO PROF.KIT MURPHY E À PROFA.DENISE PELA OPORTUNIDADE E PELA CONFIANÇA NA

MINHA CAPACIDADE EM DESENVOLVER ESTE PROJETO.

-À FAMÍLIA PELO APOIO AO LONGO DESTA JORNADA.

-AOS AMIGOS DO IBILCE QUE SEMPRE ESTIVERAM DISPOSTOS A AJUDAR.PRINCIPALMENTE

À ESTELA RODRIGUES PINTO, MUITO OBRIGADA!!!!

(6)

WOULD SING THEIR SONGS TO THE SILVERY MOON. TENORS SINGERS WERE OUT OF PLACE,

FOR EVERY FROG WAS A DOUBLE BASS. BUT NEVER A HUMAN CHORUS YET

COULD BEAT THE ACCURATE TIME THEY SET. THE SOLO SINGER BEGAN THE JOKE;

HE SANG,“AS LONG AS I’LL LIVE I’LL CROAK, CROAK,I’LL CROAK,”

AND THE CHORUS FOLLOWED HIM:“CROAK, CROAK, CROAK!”

EXERTO DE FROGS IN CHORUS,

(7)

RESUMO

Em anuros, a manutenção de espaçamento entre machos constitui uma importante adaptação às agregações reprodutivas, minimizando o mascaramento auditivo e aumentando o sucessoreprodutivo dos machos emissores de vocalização. O canto de anúncio transmite informações com o potencial de influenciar o estabelecimento e a manutenção de espaçamento entre machos, como a habilidade de luta e a localização dos machos. Avaliamos o espaçamento entre machos de Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae), que emitem canto de anúncio formado por duas notas: as introdutórias (notas A) e as secundárias (notas B). Testamos três hipóteses: 1) o espaçamento entre machos é influenciado por um ou mais parâmetros do canto de anúncio; 2) o canto de anúncio transmite informações a respeito do tamanho corporal dos machos; 3) o espaçamento entre machos é mediado por notas introdutórias. Machos de D. nanus utilizam ambas as notas para advertir seu tamanho corporal e sua localização no habitat reprodutivo. No entanto, somente as notas A transmitem ambas as informações por meio dos mesmos parâmetros acústicos, relativos à estrutura da nota. As notas B transmitem as informações relativas ao tamanho corporal pela taxa de repetição de notas e de pulsos, enquanto o espaçamento foi influenciado pelo número e duração dos pulsos. Além disso, o espaçamento entre machos foi maior quando mediado por notas A do que por notas B. Assim, as notas A informam a coespecíficos a habilidade competitiva do macho emissor da vocalização, enquanto as notas B advertem a qualidade reprodutiva dos machos às fêmeas.

(8)

ABSTRACT

In anurans, the maintenance of spacing between males is an important adaptation to

the chorus, reducing auditory masking and maximizing the transmission of the calls

and males’ reproductive success. Advertisement call conveys information with the

potential to influence the establishment and maintenance of intermale spacing, like

competitive ability and location of males. We evaluate intermale spacing in natural

aggregation of Dendropsophus nanus (Anura, Hylidae), whose advertisement call is

constituted by two notes: the introductory (type A notes) and the secondary (type B

notes). We tested three hypotheses: 1) intermale spacing is influenced by one or more

parameters of the advertisement call; 2) the advertisement call contains information

about male body size; 3) intermale spacing is mediated by introductory notes. Males of

D. nanus use both notes to advertise their body size and their location in the

reproductive habitat to other conspecifics. However, only A notes convey both kinds of

information through the same acoustic parameters, which are related to note structure.

Type B notes advertise body size through note and pulses repetition rate, while spacing

is associated to the number and duration of pulses. Furthermore, intermale spacing

was greater when mediated by A notes than when mediated by B notes. Therefore, A

notes advertise mainly a male’s competitive ability in male-male interactions, while B

notes appear to advertise the males’ reproductive quality to females.

Keywords: Anura, Bioacustics, Intermale spacing, Advertisement call, Acoustic

(9)

SUMÁRIO

Introduction ... 10

Material and methods ... 13

Study area ... 13

Study species ... 13

Relationship among advertisement call parameters, body size and male spacing ... 14

General procedures ... 14

Bioacustics analysis ... 15

Relationship between note type and male spacing ... 16

Statistical analysis ... 18

Results ... 20

Discussion ... 23

Tables ... 30

Figures ... 36

(10)

INTRODUCTION

Males of many anuran species aggregate in choruses of highly variable density

where they emit advertisement calls to attract females and mediate aggressive

interactions (Wells 1977, Duellman and Trueb 1986, Brenowitz and Rose 1994,

Gerhardt 1994, Haddad 1995). In these aggregations, animals must be able to detect

and recognize conspecific signals, localize signalers, discriminate signal types and

signalers, and extract information from signals and interactions among multiple con-

and heterospecific signalers (Gerhardt 1992, Gerhardt and Bee 2007, Wells and

Schwartz 2007). However, high level of noise, mostly biological (sounds from other

animals), impairs the detection of a signal and the transmission of its information (e.g.

Forrest 1994, Wollerman & Wiley 2002, Bee 2008, Bee and Micheyl 2008, Richardson

and Lengagne 2009).

In anurans, the maintenance of spacing between males may reduce auditory

masking from the sounds of the chorus, therefore maximizing the transmission of the

calls and males’ reproductive success (e.g. Brenowitz et al. 1984, Schwartz 1987,

Ryan 1988, Rose and Brenowitz 1991, Bee 2007, Bee and Micheyl 2008). For

example, Richardson and Lengagne (2009) investigated the effect of spacing on

females’ ability to discriminate among competing calling males within a general high

background noise setting. Through phonotaxis experiments with Hyla arborea, the

authors demonstrated that the discrimination of more attractive male calls by females

was improved with the increase of spatial separation of speakers. Although it is

possible that an optimum pattern of spacing exists, with males occupying calling sites

from where signals emitted by near conspecifics are just barely audible (Brenowitz et

al. 1984, Brenowitz and Rose 1994), which would make competition weaker and

information transmission more effective, such optimal spacing is rarely possible

(11)

spacing (Brenowitz et al. 1984, Gerhardt et al. 1989, Dyson & Passmore 1992,

Shepard 2002).

Several studies of male-male interactions demonstrated that males may adjust

their calling behavior in response to the proximity and vocalization of neighboring

conspecifics either by changing the timing of calls or by changing the structure of the

call (rate, duration, or complexity) (e.g. Bosch and Márquez 1996, 2001, Schwartz

2001, Marshall et al. 2003, Owen and Gordon 2005, Tárano and Fuenmayor 2009,

Bates et al. 2010). Yet most of the research on intermale spacing has focused on the

examination of the relationship between spacing and one advertisement call trait: call

amplitude (e.g. Brenowitz et al. 1984, Telford 1985, Gerhardt et al. 1989, Rose and

Brenowitz 1991, Stewart and Bishop 1994, Murphy and Floyd 2005). Since amplitude

attenuates with increasing distance, this trait potentially allows the receiver to assess

the distance of the competing male in the chorus: comparatively, the call of a nearer

male will be perceived by the receiver as having the higher amplitude, whereas the call

of a distant male will be perceived as having a lower call amplitude (e.g. Brenowitz and

Rose 1994, Robertson 1984).

Males are able to mediate spacing through vocalizations. Advertisement calls

traits convey diverse information about the signaler that might influence the spatial

distribution of anuran males, such as signaler’s body size: as the competitive ability

and the outcome of a fight between two individuals are highly dependent on the body

size (Parker 1974, Robertson 1986, Wagner 1992, Bee et al. 1999, Bee and Gerhardt

2001), the assessment of an opponent’s size is an important piece of information for

males to determine from the signals of rivals. Body size may be assessed through

dominant or fundamental frequencies, amplitude, call duration, number of pulses, and

pulse repetition rate (e.g. Robertson 1986, McClelland et. al. 1996, Bee et al. 2000,

Poole and Murphy 2007). Additionally, males of many anuran species may also be able

to mediate spacing through the production of two distinct note types (Littlejohn and

(12)

convey separate messages to males and females: the introductory notes often convey

aggressive messages to competing males, while secondary notes function primarily to

attract conspecific females (e.g. Narins and Capranica 1976, 1978, Littlejohn and

Harrison 1985). Advertisement calls consisting of two or more notes are common

among some groups of hylids (Wells 1988) such as Dendropsophus microcephalus

species group. This group includes Dendropsophus nanus, a South American hylid frog

whose advertisement call is formed by two simple pulsed notes: the introductory (type

A notes) and the secondary (type B notes) (Martins & Jim 2003). Martins and Jim

(2003) hypothesized that these notes perform distinct functions: as A notes are emitted

more frequently in the beginning of the chorus activity, when few males are calling, they

should be responsible for establishment and maintenance of intermale spacing, while B

notes would function to attract females as males start to emit this notes after they

establish a calling site.

In this study, we evaluated the intermale spacing between nearest males of D. nanus. Three hypotheses were tested. The first one is that spacing between males of D. nanus in the chorus is influenced by one or more parameters of the advertisement

call. This hypothesis predicts that one or more of these parameters should be

correlated with the distance between calling males and their nearest neighbors. The

second hypothesis is that the advertisement call contains information about male body

size. This hypothesis predicts that one or more parameters of calls should be

correlated with male body size. To test these hypotheses, we conducted a field study in

which we measured parameters of calls and intermale spacing in natural aggregations.

The third hypothesis is that intermale spacing is mediated by introductory notes. We

tested this hypothesis by conducting a playback experiment, in which measured the

nearest-neighbor distance to speakers broadcasting either synthetic A or B notes. It

was expected that the distance between the speaker and the nearest male would be

longer for the speaker broadcasting synthetic A notes than the speaker broadcasting

(13)

MATERIAL AND METHODS

Study area

Fieldwork was carried out during the rainy and anuran breeding season, from

January to March 2010 and from December 2010 to March 2011, on farmland located

in Macaubal (20°44’29”S; 49°56’07”W), northwestern São Paulo State, Brazil. The

region’s climate is Aw Köppen-Geiger climate type, which is characterized by hot

and wet summer (October to March) and dry winter (April to September) (CEPAGRI

2010). The annual rainfall varies from 1200 to 1650 mm (Carvalho and Assad 2005),

and the onset of the rainy season varies each year (Rossa-Feres and Jim 2001). The

original vegetation cover of Mesophitic Semideciduous Forest (Atlantic Forest Domain)

with patches of Cerrado (Ab’Saber 2003) was intensively deforested for agricultural

activities, and the remaining fragments of original vegetation are few and small (São

Paulo 2000; Rodrigues et al. 2008).

Study species

Dendropsophus nanus is widely distributed throughout South America, from

Northeastern Brazil southward through central Paraguay, northern Argentina, eastern

Bolivia, to extreme southern Brazil, Uruguay, and La Plata Basin in Argentina (Frost

2011). D. nanus is found only in open areas and is very abundant in the study region,

breeding in both temporary and permanent water bodies throughout the rainy season

(Menin 2002, Vasconcelos and Rossa-Feres 2005). Males call from the ground,

shrubs, grasses, and cattails (Bernarde and Kokubum 1999, Menin 2002, Vasconcelos

and Rossa-Feres 2005, present study). The advertisement call of D. nanus consists in

two notes (type A and type B; Figure 1) emitted in consecutive series. Notes have four

to five spectral peaks (the fundamental and three or four upper harmonics) and the

(14)

(fundamental frequency). Harmonics are less distinctive in A notes than in B notes

(Figures 2). Both notes differ significantly (p < 0.05) in their temporal structure (Table

1): type A notes are longer, with lower note repetition rate (notes/second), higher pulse

number, lower pulse duration and higher pulse repetition rate (pulses/second) than type

B notes. Notes also differ (p < 0.05) in sound pressure level (root-mean-square sound

pressure level [RMS SPL], re 20 μPa) and minimum frequency (Table 1), but do not

differ (p > 0.05) in both fundamental and maximum frequencies.

Relationship among advertisement call parameters, body size and male spacing

General procedures

Two permanent (a 9498 m2 lake and a 1074 m2 pond) and one temporary (19

m2) water bodies were visited weekly. Males started to call shortly after sunset, and the

sampling was initiated 30 minutes after the beginning of calling activity, when the

chorus was already stabilized and males were emitting both A and B notes. The

sampling period each night lasted about four hours, and males were selected for

recording at random. Fifty notes of each the calling males (n = 102) were recorded at a

sampling rate of 44100 Hz, 24 bits/sample in mono pattern, and in wave format (.wav)

using a digital recorder Edirol R-09HR coupled to a directional Sennheiser ME66/K3U

microphone positioned at a distance of 1 meter from the male. Air temperature and

humidity were measured with a digital thermohygrometer Minipa MT-240; recordings

were made at a temperature of 23.35 ± 1.14°C (mean ± SD) and relative humidity of

82.77 ± 4.22%. The angle at which the microphone was positioned in relation to the

snout of the calling male was registered; this angle varied from 0 to 280°. To avoid

recording the same male repeatedly within a night, calling sites were marked with a

numbered stake to allow identification of the focal male throughout the sampling period.

After the recording, each male was photographed with an Olympus μ 790 SW

(15)

pattern of marks on his back (Figure 3), and we compared these patterns (location,

shape and size) using the software program I³S Manta 2.1 (Hartog & Reijns 2008). In

this software, the user identifies the most distinguishing marks of each image and

stores this pattern in the database. To identify a previously photographed individual, the

pattern of marks of a new image is matched with the patterns of all the known animals

in the database and a ranked list of images is presented. We then compared the new

image with the images ranked by the software to determine if a match existed. If a

match did not exist, the image was entered into the database as a new individual.

Photos and the software program ImageJ 1.42q (Rasband 2009) were used to

obtain snout-vent length (SVL) as a measure of male body size. A ruler was positioned

beside each male when his photo was taken, and the ruler provided a known distance

from which we were able to define a spatial scale in pixel per measurement unit. The

software used the defined scale to calculate the SVL in calibrated units (for example,

centimeters). We adopted this approach, rather than measuring males directly, to avoid

disturbing the males and causing them to retreat from their calling sites.

After recordings, intermale spacing was measured as the distance between the

focal male and his nearest neighbor. Focal male perch height were also determined

because perch height influences sound propagation, with sounds attenuating more

severely when emitted near ground than when emitted from elevated perches (e.g.

Greer & Wells 1980, Wells and Schwartz 1982, Mitchell and Miller 1991); degradation

of the quality of a call may influence the effectiveness of a males’ calls in maintaining

spacing. The total number of males calling at the peak activity of the chorus was

determined each sampled night because average distances between males

necessarily decrease with increasing density of calling males (Brenowitz et al. 1984,

Gerhardt et al. 1989, Dyson & Passmore 1992, Shepard 2002).

(16)

Advertisement calls were analyzed using Raven Pro 1.3 (Cornell Bioacoustics

Research Program, 2008). Oscillograms (Figure 4), spectrograms (Figure 5) and power

spectrum views (Figure 6) were generated to measure the following parameters for A

and B notes: note amplitude (sound pressure level, SPL), minimum and maximum

frequency of the note (the frequency at the beginning and at the end of the note,

respectively), fundamental frequency (the lowest harmonic in the frequency spectrum),

dominant frequency (frequency containing the greatest energy), note duration (time

from the beginning to the end of a note), note repetition rate (number of notes per

second), number of pulses per note, pulse duration (time from the beginning to the end

of a pulse) and pulse repetition rate (number of pulses per second). As fundamental

and dominant frequencies coincided, we will refer to these frequencies just as

fundamental frequency from now on. SPL was calculated from RMS Amplitude

measures obtained from Raven of a 500 Hz tone broadcast by a speaker and recorded

for 30 seconds with the digital recorder and the microphone positioned at 1 meter

distance. Recordings were made for SPL values varying from 70 to 100 dB, and

measures were made in intervals of 3 dB SPL with a RadioShack Sound Level Meter

33-2055SPL. A calibration curve was constructed from these measures by fitting an

exponential function (y = 33.698e0.1129x, where x represents SPL values and y

represents RMS amplitude values). With this equation it was possible to back-calculate

SPL values from the RMS amplitudes measured from D. nanus advertisement calls.

Relationship between note type and male spacing

To test the hypothesis that the intermale spacing is mediated by A notes, we

synthesized both A and B notes utilizing the average values of acoustic parameters

obtained from the bioacoustic analysis of D. nanus natural calls recorded during the study’s first rainy season. All values used represent the average for the population for

that parameter. The A note lasted 35 ms and consisted of twelve pulses and four

(17)

fourth harmonics attenuated relative to the fundamental by -31.77, -40.86, -45.28 dB.

The B note lasted 19 ms and consisted of six pulses and four harmonics, with a

fundamental frequency of 4.32 kHz and the second through the fourth harmonics

attenuated relative to the fundamental by -33.38, -38.92, -45.30 dB. Synthetic calls

were produced at a sampling rate of 44.10 kHz and depth of 24 bits/sample in mono

pattern with the software Audacity 1.2.6 and were stored as digital files (.wav) on the

digital recorder.

Synthetic calls were played from the recorder through an amplifier and

broadcast from two speakers placed randomly in the study water bodies. We broadcast

synthetic calls at two different alternated sequences, one was constituted by A notes

and the other by B notes, following the natural temporal properties of calling activity.

Because note repetition rate was correlated to the temperature in natural choruses (r =

0.39, p < 0.05), the specific note repetition rate was obtained from the regression of

repetition rate on temperature. Three value of temperature were selected for the

playback generation of repetition rates, representing the central (24.5°C) and end

points (22.5 and 26.5°C) of the range of natural temperatures: 1) for temperature close

to 22.5°C, notes were repeated at a rate of 0.85 notes/s, 2) at 24.5°C, the note

repetition rate was 1 note/s, and 3) at 26.5°C, 1.10 notes/s were emitted from

speakers. When conducting playbacks the note repetition rate selected was the one

whose temperature was the most similar to the temperature at the study site at the

beginning of the playback experiment.

The playback experiment tested the spacing of arriving males relative to

established residents, with speakers simulating resident males. Playback began with

sunset, before males arrived at the study water body each night. The experiment was

conducted on 20 nights (10 nights in each of the two permanent water bodies of water)

and was carried out until the chorus stabilized. Each night the two note types were

randomly assigned to the two speakers, with one speaker broadcasting note type A and

(18)

environmental variables (e.g. temperature) that might affect male responses. During

the experiment, the distance between the speaker and the nearest male was

determined with the use of a tape measure (to the nearest 1 mm). We recorded the

positions of calling males relative to the speakers every 15 minutes. Also, we counted

the total number of calling males during the censuses as intermale spacing varies with

chorus density.

Statistical analyses

Because recorded males were identified individually, each male (n = 100) was

treated as an independent subject. For males recorded multiple times, only the first

recording was used. Acoustic data were expressed as the mean calculated from the 30

calls analyzed for each male. Although recordings were made after chorus stabilization,

two of the 102 recorded males only emitted type A notes and were therefore excluded

from analyses. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied to verify whether differences

in number, perch height and distance between calling males existed between

permanents and temporary ponds. Data were normally distributed, and all the statistical

analyses were conducted in R 2.13.2 (R Development Core Team 2010).

To check for significant associations (p < 0.05) between the analyzed acoustic

parameters and microphone angle, air temperature and relative humidity, we calculated

Spearman rank correlation coefficients (r). Humidity was significant associated with

duration (r = 0.24, p = 0.03) and number (r = -0.23, p = 0.03) of pulses of type A notes,

and duration (r = 0.22, p = 0.05), fundamental frequency (r = 0.22, p = 0.04), and

maximum frequency (r = -0.22, p = 0.05) of type B notes. Microphone angle was

significantly associated with fundamental frequency (r = -0.22, p = 0.05) and maximum

(r = -0.27, p = 0.01) frequencies of B notes. Because of these associations, observed

values of acoustic parameters’ were converted to residual values, which were obtained

(19)

microphone angle). These residual values were used in subsequent statistical

analyses.

PCA based on correlation matrix (Tables 2 and 3) was applied to reduce the

dimensionality of the data (Budaev 2010, Legendre & Legendre 1998). Through PCA,

highly correlated variables are linearly combined and represented by statistical

variables or Principal Components (PCs). Variables highly correlated (i.e. presenting

high loadings) with the same PC may be considered as having the same cause.

Loadings were considered significant (p < 0.05) when they exceeded ± 0.55 (Hair et al.

2009). Communalities were calculated as the sum of the squared loadings of a variable

in relation to each PC and show how much variance of a variable is explained by the

factorial solution, i.e. the PCs taken together (Hair et al. 2009).

To examine whether the data were appropriate for the application of Principal

Component Analysis (PCA), two tests were applied: 1) the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO)

index, which measures the sampling adequacy and should be greater than 0.5 for a

satisfactory factor analysis to proceed (Hair et al. 2009, Budaev 2010), and 2) Bartlett's

sphericity test to verify whether the correlation matrix is an identity matrix, which would

indicate that the factor model is inappropriate (Bartlett, 1937; Hair et al. 2009, Budaev

2010). Results from Bartlett’s sphericity test rejected the hypotheses of null correlation

(type A notes: χ2= 501.75, df = 36, p = < 0.00001; type B notes: χ2= 440.93, df = 36, p =

< 0.00001) and KMO indicated that the sample size is appropriated for PCA (type A

notes = 0.602, type B notes = 0.55).

The number of Principal Components (PCs) to be retained was determined with

Horn’s Parallel Analysis (PA; Horn 1965), which contrasts eigenvalues produced

through a PCA on a number of random data sets of uncorrelated variables with the

same number of variables and observations as the observational dataset to produce

eigenvalues for components that are adjusted for the sample error-induced inflation

(20)

according to the Kaiser’s rule must be retained (Horn 1965, Franklin et al. 1995, Dinno

2009). PA was applied using a 95% confidence interval.

We used multiple regression analysis to assess the statistical significance of

each retained component as a predictor of (1) distance among calling males and (2)

male body size. The total number of calling males in each sampled night and the perch

height of recorded calling males were included in each analysis as covariates. From a

global model, i.e. one that incorporates all variables of interest, were generated

submodels by the exclusion of independent variables that were non-significant (p >

0.05), i.e. in each model, variables with the highest value of p were subsequently

excluded until the model was composed only by significant variables. To compare the

submodels and identify the one that best fit the observed data, a second-order Akaike

Information Criteria (AICc) was used; the best fitting submodel is the one with the

smallest value of AICc (Burnham and Anderson 2002).

To evaluate whether A notes function to maintain intermale spacing, we included

the distance between the speaker and nearest calling male as the response variable,

with the note type (A or B) as a within-night factor, the water body in which the playback

experiment was conducted as a between-night factor, and the number of calling males

as a covariate in a repeated measures statistical analysis. We applied Linear Mixed

Effects Model, in which the night was considered a random component, while the note

type, water body, and number of calling males were considered fixed components of

the analysis. As a significantly difference in number of males was detected between the

two bodies of water, we also tested the interaction between the variables. The

assumptions of normality, homocedasticity and sphericity were violated, so we assume

a more conservative significant value (p < 0.01).

RESULTS

Calling males of Dendropsophus nanus had a body size of 21.10 ± 2.20 mm

(21)

among water bodies (F2,97 = 8.13, p < 0.001): intermale spacing in the permanent water

bodies was significantly higher than in the temporary pond (post-hoc Tukey test, p <

0.001; Table 4). The number of calling males in each sampled night was highly variable

and different among water bodies (F2,97 = 12.76, p < 0.0001). More males were

recorded at the lake than the permanent pond, and more were recorded at the

permanent pond than at the temporary pond. The lake averaged more calling males

per night than did the permanent pond (post-hoc Tukey test, p < 0.0001; Table 4) but

no difference was detected between the permanent and temporary ponds (post-hoc

Tukey test, p > 0.05). Differences among bodies of water were also detected in perch

height (F2,97 = 11.68, p < 0.0001): calling males adopted lower perch height in

temporary water bodies, where only grasses were available to be used as calling sites,

than in permanent water bodies (post-hoc Tukey test, p < 0.001; Table 4), where

shrubs, grasses and cattails were available for perches.

PCA for the acoustic parameters of type A notes reduced the original nine

variables to four factors that accounted for 75.51% of the observed variation (Table 5).

PC1 represented parameters associated with the structure of the note emitted by the

signaler: duration of notes, number of pulses, and pulse repetition rate loaded

positively, and duration of pulses loaded negatively. Taken together, parameters

grouped by this factor indicated that males produced longer notes by increasing the

number of pulses while simultaneously shorting the duration of each pulse. PC2 was

negatively associated with maximum frequency and positively associated with note

repetition rate (Table 5), indicating that males will call at higher rates when maximum

frequency is lower. PC3 grouped fundamental and minimum frequencies, both of which

were positively associated with this factor (Table 5). PC4 was associated positively with

SPL (Table 5). These four factors were then renamed accordingly to the parameters

that loaded significantly onto these factors: Note Structure (= PC1), Note Rate (= PC2),

Frequency (= PC3) and Amplitude (= PC4). We used these factors as the predictor

(22)

For B notes, five PCs were retained, and they explained 85.73% of the variance

(Table 6). PC1 grouped two acoustic parameters with significant loadings: duration and

number of pulses. The first is negatively, and the second is positively associated with

PC1 (Table 6); i.e. the B note consists of a high number of short pulses. Balancing the

number and duration of pulses, males determine the structure of the note emitted. PC2

was positively associated with repetition rate of notes and pulses (Table 6). PC3 was

positively associated with the minimum frequency. PC4 was significantly associated

with SPL and maximum frequency. Duration of notes was positively associated with

PC5. These factors were renamed as: Note Structure, Repetition Rate, Minimum

Frequency, Amplitude, and Note Duration, respectively.

Regression models to assess the statistical significance of each PC for note

type A as a predictor of distance between calling males showed that, although both

PC1 (Note Structure) and PC2 (Note Rate) components were incorporated to the best

fit model (R = 0.23; Adjusted R² = 0.20; F4,95 = 7.31; p < 0.0001; AICc: 91.45; AICc

weight: 0.344), only the first PC was statistically significant (Table 7). This result

indicates that males who produced type A notes with fewer, longer pulses within shorter

notes were more distant from the neighbors than males that produced type A notes with

more, short pulses within longer notes. For parameters related to B notes, only

Repetition Rate was incorporated into the model (R = 0.21; Adjusted R² = 0.18; F3,96 =

8.49; p < 0.0001; AICc: 92.50; AICc weight: 0.60), and it significantly predicted spacing

(Table 8). Perch height and number of males calling in the chorus were also important

predictors of intermale spacing: perch height was positively, while the number of males

was negatively, associated with the spacing (Tables 7 and 8).

Body size of males, measured as Snout Vent Length (SVL), was predicted by

three of the four retained PCs related to the parameters of A notes(R = 0.16; Adjusted

R²= 0.13; F3.96 = 6.11; p = 0.0007; AICc = -33.25; AICc weight = 0.451; Table 9), but just

the PC1 (Note Structure) and PC2 (Note Rate) were statistically significant predictors in

(23)

= 0.08; Adjusted R²= 0.06; F2.97 = 4.08; p = 0.02; AICc = -26.07; AICc weight = 0.304)

but only Note Structure predicted significantly males SVL (Table 10). In both cases, the

best fit models incorporated non-significant PCs.

The type of note broadcasted and the number of male calling each night had a

significant effect on the distance between the speaker and the nearest calling male,

while no effect of water body or the interaction between water body and the number of

males in the chorus was detect (Table 11). Calling males were closest to the speaker

broadcasting B notes than to the speaker broadcasting A notes (Figure 7): the mean

distance between speaker broadcasting A notes and nearest male was 2.49 ± 2.16 m,

while males around the speaker broadcasting B notes adopted calling sites at a

distance of 1.93 ± 0.95 m. An average of 11.03 ± 7.36 males was present in the chorus

each night. The total number of males in the chorus influenced the resulting intermale

spacing as short distances separated calling males when many were present, while

males were far apart when the chorus was formed by few males (Figure 8).

DISCUSSION

In this study, we investigated which advertisement call parameters and which

type of note convey information related to the maintenance of the intermale spacing

and to the competitive ability relative to male’s body size.

The characteristics of the studied water bodies influenced the number of calling

males, the intermale spacing and the perch heights adopted by D. nanus. In the lake, it

was registered an average number of calling males significantly greater than the

permanent and temporary ponds, but the highest number of males did not result in

shorter distances between calling males despite the expected decline of intermale

spacing with the increase of the number of calling males and density in the chorus

(Brenowitz et al. 1984, Gerhardt et al. 1989, Dyson & Passmore 1992, Shepard 2002).

Additionally, although the number of males in the permanent and temporary ponds did

(24)

smaller than at the permanent pond. These results may reflect (1) differences in the

size of water bodies: the shoreline area available for establishment of calling sites is

much larger at the lake than at the permanent pond, and the shoreline available for

occupancy at the permanent pond is larger than the area at the temporary pond or (2)

low availability of perches in the temporary pond for males establish calling sites. Since

males were found calling perched and also on the ground level, the availability of

perches did not seem to restrict the occurrence of calling males at each studied water

body, but where high-growing vegetation, such as shrubs and cattails, was available

(permanent water bodies), males were frequently found using these vegetation types

as calling sites and, therefore, calling from elevated positions. Calling from an elevated

position may enhance sounds propagation: when the signaler is elevated above the

ground, the attenuating effect of the ground surface is reduced, and the effective

distance that the sound may propagate is increased (e.g. Mitchell and Miller 1990,

Forrest 1994, Parris 2002). Thus, the area over which the call will effectively prevent a

closer approach by rivals is increased, and males will space themselves more widely.

Although the effects of the number of calling males and perch height on intermale

spacing varied with the water body characteristics, the results of model selection

indicate that these variables play an important role in determining spacing along with

acoustic parameters of both introductory (A) and secondary (B) notes.

Parameters of A notes that grouped in the first component (Note Structure) were

significantly related to intermale spacing, indicating their importance as predictors of

nearest-neighbor distance. Duration of notes, number and duration of pulses, and

pulses repetition rate are parameters related to the calling effort of signalers and may

indicate to competitors the male’s motivational state to fight, as emission of longer

notes by males is generally associated with aggressive interactions (e.g. Wells &

Schwartz 1984, Wells 1988, Lesbarrères and Lodé 2002). For example, Wells &

Schwartz (1984) verified that male of Hyla ebraccata present graded response to the

(25)

Besides, the production of notes of longer duration helps to increase the signal to noise

ratio of males vocalization, which make them more conspicuous to conspecifics (Wells

1988, Gerhardt 1992), indicating that the lengthening of the call in response to

competitors may function to balance the competition for mates and agonistic behaviors

(Wells 1988, Wagner 1989). However, though these researches suggest that

parameters related to the note structure mediate aggressive interactions and,

therefore, would be correlated to reduced territories, we found a negative relationship

between note structure and spacing, indicating that males who emit longer notes did

not hold larger territories and are closer to other males than its competitors who emit

shorter notes. Taking together with this finding the result of SVL modeling, which

indicates that A note structure is positively associated to body size, we verified that the

larger males were responsible for the production of the longer notes. As males with

larger body sizes may obtain more food than the smaller ones, which guarantee energy

enough to allow them to sustain longer duration of signaling (Gerhardt 1992). As high

calling effort involves the expenditure of great amount of energy (Ryan 1988), it is

expected that this calling behavior be associated with larger males. As males’ body size

can be assessed through the A note structure, other males in the chorus are allowed to

evaluate its ability to fight and win combats (Robertson 1986). If the larger male is a

superior competitor in comparison to the male doing the evaluation, then aggressive

attacks against it will be avoided. So, larger males may occupy suitable calling sites

independently of the closeness of other males, tolerating their presence while they

offered no threats to its position in the chorus. This may be a better strategy than

engage into a fight at the expense of energy which may be allocated to reproduction.

The second component of spacing being modeled by parameters of A note

(Note Rate) also was incorporated in the best fit model that explained intermale

spacing but it was not a statistically significant predictor. Note repetition rate is a

parameter related to the calling effort of males and may provide reliable information

(26)

indicate the physiological condition of the calling male (Gerhardt 1992, Schwartz 1994,

Schwartz 2001, Schwartz et al. 2002). Another acoustic parameter was grouped in this

component: the maximum frequency. Signals of low frequency propagate through

longer distances (Gerhardt 1994) and, for many species, are able to indicate callers

body size to coespecifics (Ramer et al. 1983, Robertson 1986). Therefore, it would be

expected that frequency should influence intermale spacing if not for its propagation

properties then for the information about the body size. However, the results of model

selection did not reveal any significant relationship among these acoustic parameters

and spacing. In relation to the body size, although call frequency is often negatively

associated with signaler size (Ramer et al. 1983, Robertson 1986), frequency

parameters were not consistently associated with body size in D. nanus. The only

frequency parameter with a significant association to SVL was the A note maximum

frequency in the PC2 (Note Rate). This parameter was negatively correlated with PC2,

while this PC was positively associated to SVL, indicating that a low maximum

frequency is related to a larger male. Other frequency parameters of A notes were

grouped in the third component of the SVL model which, although not significantly

associated to the SVL, presented a negative association with body size (i.e. high

values of minimum and fundamental frequencies indicate smaller male) and

contributed to the model increasing its predictive power. So, note repetition rate convey

information about the physiological condition, while maximum frequency informs about

body size of males. Probably these parameters are used by the females in the

assessment of the reproductive fitness of the male.

About the acoustic parameters of B notes, a positive association of parameters

grouped in the PC2 (Repetition Rate) with the intermale spacing indicate that the

distance between nearest calling males will be greater as the note and pulse repetition

rate increases. Note repetition rate is frequently adjusted as a result of male–male

competition in ways that may help to maintain or increase the male’s relative

(27)

repetition rate has the potential to transmit information about species and individual

identity (Gerhardt 1992) and also may mediate aggressive interactions (Wells 1988).

So, these parameters may be used by conspecific to assess which individual in the

chorus is calling and how high is its motivation to fight and breed, with males spacing

themselves as a way to increase their ability to attract females through the calling

activity and the properties of their calls. In relation to body size, only the PC1 (Note

Structure) was significantly and positively related to SVL, indicating that larger males

produced B notes with shorter and more numerous pulses than smaller males. In

accordance with what it was found to A notes acoustic parameters, also for B notes

frequency parameters did not encode information about body size. This result may

indicate that other acoustic parameters are more effective predictors of body size in D. nanus than is frequency.

Several researches have demonstrated that different advertisement call

parameters may convey different information about the signalers. Measures of

frequency may provide a reliable indicative of the body size, related to fight ability (e.g.

Ramer et al. 1983, Robertson 1986). Note repetition rate reflects, in aggressive

contexts, the motivation of the resident male to defend its territory or attack a

competitor, while in reproductive contexts, allows females to infer the reproductive

fitness of the signaling male since maintenance of high repetition rates may be costly to

the male (Gerhardt 1992, Welch et al. 1998, Schwartz 2001). SPL is a parameter that

may allow males to assess the distance of the signaler (e.g. Robertson 1984). The

pulse repetition rate is a static property of the call with the potential to transmit

information about specific and individual identity (Gerhardt 1992). Duration of notes

constitutes an honest indicator of male genetic quality potentially influencing patterns of

female mate choice (Welch et al. 1998). So, the verified association of parameters of

both note types A and B with intermale distance may reflect the different functions of

these parameters and notes in the establishment and maintenance of spacing. Also,

(28)

from a combination of parameters that reflect both notes structure. According to

Gerhardt (1992), if the values of different parameters are highly correlated, then such

redundancy may contribute to a more reliable extraction of the encoded information.

The relationship among acoustic parameters of A notes, spacing and body size

indicate that components related to the distance between calling males are also

important predictors of SVL of D. nanus. Both factor components included in the SVL

model were also included in the spacing model, but just one (Note Structure) was a

statistically significant predictor of SVL. Therefore, it seems that the same A note

parameters are responsible for conveying both kind of information in a way that

spacing may reflect not only the acoustic parameters and their propagation but also the

body size of calling males. It is possible to infer that the establishment of intermale

spacing at the beginning of the chorus, characterized by males emitting A notes, occurs

through the assessment of the competitive ability, relative to the body size, of rivals

encoded in the calls emitted. On the contrary, the parameters of B notes associated

with the spacing are not related to SVL, conveying just the information of location and

distance between competitors, while information about the body size is encoded in

other parameters. Therefore, in relation to spacing, parameters of A and B notes

convey different information: while the A note informs competitors about fight ability,

mainly related to males’ SVL, B note advertise the males reproductive fitness to

females.

The results of the playback experiment indicate that the type of note emitted

influences how males spaces throughout the reproductive habitat: males assumed

calling position farther from the speaker broadcasting A notes than the speaker

broadcasting B notes. So, although results of the advertisement call parameters

analysis indicate that both notes convey information that affects intermale spacing in D. nanus, the playback experiment reveals that A notes may be more effective in

determining spacing than B notes. This result confirm the hypothesis that D. nanus A

(29)

Considering a male-male interaction, it is possible that, at the beginning of chorus

activity, males emit A notes to inform their location and competitive ability to other male

who may use this information in the establishment of its calling site, ultimately resulting

in the establishment of intermale spacing, while emission of B notes during the chorus

activity may reinforce the message.

In summary, males of Dendropsophus nanus use acoustic signals to advertise

their body size and their location in the reproductive habitat to other conspecifics which

may assess the fighting ability and the distance of the calling male. This information is

transmitted through both notes that compose the advertisement call of this species.

Parameters of A notes convey both kinds of information, whereas parameters of B

notes affect male spacing but do not convey information about male body size.

Therefore, A notes appear to advertise mainly a male’s competitive ability in male-male

interactions, whereas B notes appear to advertise the males’ reproductive quality to

females and the different information conveyed by the different notes reflects in the

intermale distance. When the distance between calling males is mediated by A notes,

males occupy sites distant from each other, whereas they adopt close positioning in the

(30)

TABLES

Table 1. Mean ± Standard Deviation (SD) of type A and B notes parameters and results from paired t-test used to compare parameters between the notes. Parameters were considered significantly different between notes when p < 0.05.

Mean ± SD of notes Paired t-test Acoustic

Parameters A B t df p

Sound Pressure

Level (dB) 86.06 ± 5.20 87.46 ± 5.23 -13.93 99 < 0.0001 Fundamental

frequency (kHz) 4.24 ± 0.18 4.25 ± 0.17 0.82 99 0.412 Minimum

frequency (kHz) 2.10 ± 0.50 2.30 ± 0.50 -7.88 99 < 0.0001 Maximum

frequency (kHz) 21.89 ± 0.23 21.85 ± 0.35 1.50 99 0.138 Duration of note

(s)

0.038 ±

0.006 0.021 ± 0.004 33.17 99 < 0.0001

Note repetition

rate (notes/s) 0.81 ± 0.34 5.91 ± 0.59 -4.07 99 < 0.0001 Duration of pulses

(s) 0.003 ± 0.0006 0.004 ± 0.001 -11.59 99 < 0.0001

Number of pulses 13.57 ± 3.96 5.91 ± 1.60 24.04 99 < 0.0001

Pulses repetition

rate (pulses/s) 10.83 ± 5.59 5.91 ± 3.11 7.81 99 < 0.0001

Table 2. Correlations among nine acoustic parameters of type A notes. Pearson correlation coefficients are given below the diagonal and P values are given above the diagonal (two-tailed, n = 100). Statistically significant (p < 0.05) correlations are indicated in bold type.

Acoustic parameters SPL FF MinF MaxF DN NRR NPN DP PRR Sound pressure level (SPL) 0.95 0.03 0.05 0.65 0.98 0.50 0.40 0.60

Fundamental frequency (FF) -0.01 0.19 0.22 0.32 0.91 0.14 0.06 0.78

Minimum frequency (MinF) -0.22 0.06 0.78 0.10 0.86 0.01 0.02 0.19

Maximum frequency (MaxF) 0.20 0.03 -0.03 0.52 0.63 0.24 0.08 0.65

(31)

Table 3. Correlations among nine acoustic parameters of type B notes. Pearson correlation coefficients are given below the diagonal and P values are given above the diagonal (two-tailed, n = 100). Statistically significant (p < 0.05) correlations are indicated in bold type.

Acoustic parameters SPL FF MinF MaxF DN NRR NPN DP PRR Sound pressure level (SPL) 0.44 0.01 0.08 0.59 0.22 0.33 0.12 0.42

Fundamental frequency (FF) -0.08 0.12 0.25 0.76 0.09 0.14 0.02 0.31

Minimum frequency (MinF) -0.27 0.25 0.58 0.89 1.00 0.02 0.89 0.34

Maximum frequency (MaxF) 0.18 0.01 0.06 0.17 0.69 0.14 0.44 0.45

Duration of notes (DN) 0.05 -0.03 0.01 0.14 0.98 0.00 0.27 0.10

Notes repetition rate (NRR) 0.12 -0.17 0.00 0.04 0.00 0.04 0.05 0.00 Number of pulses/note (NPN) -0.10 0.15 -0.23 0.15 0.50 -0.21 0.00 0.13

Duration of pulses (DP) 0.16 -0.24 0.23 -0.08 0.11 0.20 -0.74 0.24

Pulses repetition rate (PRR) 0.08 -0.10 -0.10 0.08 0.17 0.85 0.15 -0.12

Table 4. Mean ± Standard deviation (SD) of 100 calling males’ perch height, intermale

spacing and total number per night.

Variables Permanent pond Lake Temporary pond

Number of recorded males 30 55 15

Perch height (m) 0.36 ± 0.17 0.39 ± 0.11 0.18 ± 0.10

Distance between calling males (m) 3.58 ± 3.92 2.74 ± 3.48 1.03 ± 0.56

(32)
(33)
(34)

Table 7. Multiple regression of the best fit model in which spacing is predicted by two of the retained PCs related to A notes emitted by males of D. nanus, perch height, and

number of males (N males). B: unstandardized coefficient; Beta: standardized coefficient; r: partial correlation coefficient.

B Std. Error Beta t(95) p r

Intercept 0.216 0.097 2.232 0.028

Note Structure (PC1) -0.066 0.023 -0.269 -2.944 0.004 -0.289

Note Rate (PC2) 0.050 0.030 0.151 1.670 0.098 0.168

Perch height 0.498 0.235 0.192 2.123 0.036 0.212

N males -0.009 0.003 -0.249 -2.725 0.008 -0.269

Table 8. Multiple regression of the best fit model in which spacing is predicted by one of the retained PCs related to B notes emitted by males of D. nanus, perch height, and

number of males (N males). B: unstandardized coefficient; Beta: standardized coefficient; r: partial correlation coefficient.

B Std. Error Beta t(96) p r

Intercept 0.169 0.099 1.707 0.091

Repetition Rate (PC2) 0.076 0.027 0.258 2.809 0.006 0.275

Perch height 0.702 0.238 0.271 2.948 0.004 0.288

N males -0.011 0.003 -0.296 -3.257 0.002 -0.315

Table 9. Best-fit model for the multiple regression of Snout Vent Length (SVL) on three of the retained PCs related to A notes emitted by males of D. nanus. B: unstandardized

coefficient; Beta: standardized coefficient; r: partial correlation coefficient.

B Std. Error Beta t(96) p r

Intercept 2.110 0.020 106.382 << 0.001

Note Structure (PC1) 0.038 0.012 0.301 3.222 0.002 0.312

Note Rate (PC2) 0.038 0.016 0.221 2.364 0.020 0.234

Frequency (PC3) -0.027 0.018 -0.144 -1.538 0.127 -0.155

Table 10. Best fit model for the multiple regression of Snout Vent Length (SVL) on two of the retained PCs related to B notes emitted by males of D. nanus. B: unstandardized

coefficient; Beta: standardized coefficient; r: partial correlation coefficient.

B Std. Error Beta t(97) p r

Intercept 2.110 0.021 102.03 << 0.001

Note structure (PC1) 0.034 0.014 0.232 2.380 0.019 0.234

(35)

Table 11. Effects of note type (A and B), number of males in the chorus, and water body where the experiment was conducted (fixed components) on the distance between speakers and the nearest calling male.

Value Std. Error df t p

Intercept 3.32 0.45 185 7.41 0.00

Note type -0.60 0.18 185 -3.30 0.001

Number of males -0.10 0.03 185 -3.01 0.003

Water Body -0.40 0.66 18 -0.61 0.551

(36)

FIGURES

Figure 1. (a) Oscilogram and (b) spectrogram of type A and B notes that compose the

(37)

Figure 2. Power spectrum of Dendropsophus nanus (a) type A and (b) B notes.

(38)

Figure 3. Two males Dendropsophus nanus showing the distinctive pattern of marks

(39)

Figure 4. Oscillograms of (a) a sequence of notes and (b) one Type A note emitted by

males of Dendropsophus nanus showing the parameters Notes Repetition Rate, Note

(40)

Figure 5. Spectrogram of a Type A note emitted by male Dendropsophus nanus,

showing the parameters measure Pulse Repetition Rate, Number of Pulses per Note

(41)

Figure 6. Power spectrum of a Type B note emitted by male of Dendropsophus nanus,

showing the parameters Fundamental Frequency (FF), minimum and maximum

frequency, and the bandwidth of frequencies.

(42)
(43)

REFERENCES

AB’SABER, A.N. Os domínios de natureza no Brasil: potencialidades paisagísticas. São

Paulo: Ateliê Editorial, 2003.

BACKWELL, P.R.Y. Functional partitioning in the two-part call of the leaf-folding frog, Afrixalus

brachycnemis. Herpetologica, v. 44, p. 1-7, 1988.

BARTLETT, M.S. Properties of sufficiency and statistical tests. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. A

Math. Phys. Sci., v. 160, n. 901, p. 268-282, 1937.

BATES, M.E. et al. Spatial location influences vocal interactions in bullfrog choruses. J.Acoust.

Soc. Am., v. 127, n. 4, p. 2664–2677,2010.

BEE, M.A. Sound source segregation in grey treefrogs: spatial release from masking by the

sound of a chorus. Anim. Behav., v. 74, n. 3, p. 549-558, 2007.

______. Finding a mate at a cocktail party: spatial release from masking improves acoustic

mate recognition in grey treefrogs. Anim. Behav., v. 75, n. 5, p. 1781-1791, 2008.

BEE, M.A.; GERHARDT, H.C. Neighbour–stranger discrimination by territorial male bullfrogs

(Rana catesbeiana): II. Perceptual basis. Anim. Behav., v. 62, n. 6, p. 1141-1150, 2001.

BEE, M.A.; MICHEYL, C. The “cocktail party problem”: what is it? How can it be solved? And

why should animal behaviorists study it? J. Comp. Psychol., v. 122, n. 3, p. 235-251, 2008.

BEE, M.A.; PERRILL, S.A.; OWEN, P.C. Male green frogs lower the pitch of acoustic signals in

defense of territories: a possible dishonest signal of size? Behav. Ecol., v. 11, n. 2, p. 169-177,

2000.

______. Size assessment in simulated territorial encounters between male green frogs (Rana

clamitans). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., v. 45, p. 177-184, 1999.

BERNARDE, P.S.; KOKUBUM, M.N.C. Anurofauna do Município de Guararapes, Estado de

São Paulo, Brasil (Amphibia, Anura). Acta. Biológica Leopoldensia, v. 21, n. 1, p. 89-97,

1999.

BOSCH, J.; MÁRQUEZ, R. Call timing in male-male acoustical interactions and female choice in

(44)

BRENOWITZ, E.A.; ROSE, G.J. Behavioural plasticity mediates aggression in choruses of the

Pacific treefog. Anim. Behav., v. 47, n. 3, p. 633-641, 1994.

BRENOWITZ, E.A.; WILCZYNSKI, W.; ZAKON, H.H. Acoustic communication in spring

peepers: enviromental and behavioral aspects. J. Comp. Physiol. A, v. 155, p. 585-592, 1984.

BUDAEV, S.V. Using principal components and factor analysis in animal behaviour research:

caveats and guidelines. Ethology, v. 116, n. 5, p. 472-480, 2010.

BURNHAM, K.P.; ANDERSON, D.R. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical

information-theoretic approach. 2nd. ed. New York: Springer-Verlag New York, Inc., 2002.

CARVALHO, J.R.P.; ASSAD, E.D. Análise espacial da precipitação pluviométrica no Estado de

São Paulo: comparação de métodos de interpolação. Eng. Agríc., v. 25, n. 2, p. 377-384, 2005.

CEPAGRI - Centro de Pesquisas Meteorológicas e Climáticas Aplicadas à Agricultura. Clima

dos municípios paulistas: a classificação climática de Köeppen para o Estado de São Paulo.

Disponível em:

<http://www.cpa.unicamp.br/outras-informacoes/clima-dos-municipios-paulistas.html>. Acesso em: 10.jan.2011.

CORNELL BIOACOUSTICS RESEARCH PROGRAM. Interactive sound analysis software:

Raven Pro 1.3. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, U.S.A. 2008. Disponível em:

<www.birds.cornell.edu/raven>.

DINNO, A. Exploring the sensitivity of Horn’s parallel analysis to the distributional form of

random data. Multivariate Behav Res., v. 44, n. 3, p. 362-388, 2009.

DUELLMAN, W.E; TRUEB, L. Biology of Amphibians. New Yok: McGraw-Hill Book Company,

1986. p. 87-108.

DYSON, M.L.; PASSMORE, N.I. Effect of intermale spacing on female frequency preferences in

the painted reed frog. Copeia, v. 1992, n. 4, p. 1111-1114. 1992

FORREST, T.G. From sender to receiver: propagation and environmental effects on acoustic

signals. Amer. Zool., v. 34, p. 644-654, 1994.

FRANKLIN, S.B. et al. Parallel Analysis: a method for determining significant principal

(45)

GERHARDT, H.C. Multiple messages in acoustic signals. Semin. Neurosci., v. 4, p. 391-400,

1992.

______. The evolution of vocalization in frogs and toads. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst., v. 25, p.

293-324, 1994.

GERHARDT, H.C.; BEE, M.A. Recognition and localization of acoustic signals. In: NARINS,

P.M. et al. (Ed.). Hearing and sound communication in amphibians. New York: Springer

Science + Business Media, LLC, 2007. p. 113-146.

GERHARDT, H.C.; DIEKAMP, B.; PTACEK, M. Inter-male spacing in choruses of the spring

peeper, Pseudacris (Hyla) crucifer. Anim. Behav., v. 38, n. 6, p. 1012-1024, 1989.

GREER, B.J.; WELLS, K.D. Territorial and reproductive behavior of the tropical american frog

Centrolenella fleischmanni. Herpetologica, v. 36, p. 318-326, 1980.

HADDAD, C.F.B. Comunicação em anuros (Amphibia). Anais de Etologia, v. 13, p. 116-132,

1995.

HAIR, J.F. et al. Análise multivariada de dados. 6. ed. São Paulo: Artmed Editora S.A., 2009.

p. 100-146.

HARTOG, J.; REIJNS, R. I³S: Interactive Individual Identification System. Versão 2.1. Boston:

Free Software Foundation, 2008.

______. I³S Manta Manual: Interactive Individual Identification System (Manta version 2.1).

Boston: Free Software Foundation, 2008. 36 p.

HORN, J.L. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrica, v.

30, n. 2, p. 179-185, 1965.

LESBARRÈRES, D.; LODÉ, T. Variations in male calls and responses to an unfamiliar

advertisement call in a territorial breeding anuran, Rana dalmatina: evidence for a dear enemy

effect. Ethology, Ecology & Evolution, v. 14, p. 287-295, 2002.

LEGENDRE, P.; LEGENDRE, L. Numerical ecology. 2nd. ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science

(46)

LITTLEJOHN, M.J.; HARRISON, P.A. The functional significance of the diphasic advertisement

call of Geocrinia victoriana (Anura: Leptodactylidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., v. 16, n. 4, p.

363-373, 1985.

MARSHALL, V.T.; HUMFELD, S.C.; BEE, M.A. Plasticity of aggressive signalling and its

evolution. Anim. Behav., v. 65, n. 6, p. 1223-1234, 2003.

MARTINS, I.A.; JIM, J. Bioacoustic analysis of advertisement call in Hyla nana and Hyla

sanborni (Anura, Hylidae) in Botucatu, São Paulo, Brazil. Braz. J. Biol., v. 63, n. 3, p. 507-516,

2003.

McCLELLAND, B.E.; WILCZYNSKI, W.; RYAN, M.J. Correlations between call characteristics

and morphology in male cricket frogs (Acris crepitans). J. Exp. Biol., v. 199, p. 1907-1919,

1996.

MENIN, M. Partilha de recursos e coexistência de populações sintópicas de Hyla nana e

Hyla sanborni (Anura, Hylidae). 2002. 30 p. Dissertação (Mestrado em Ecologia e

Conservação de Recursos Naturais) – Universidade Federal de Uberlândia, Uberlândia, 2002.

MITCHELL, S.L.; MILLER, G.L. Intermale spacing and calling site characteristics in a southern

Mississippi chorus of Hyla cinerea. Copeia, v. 1991, n. 2, p. 521-524, 1991.

MURPHY, C.G.; FLOYD, S.B. The effect of call amplitude on male spacing in choruses of

barking treefrogs, Hyla gratiosa. Anim. Behav., v. 69, n. 2, p. 419-426, 2005.

NARINS, P.M.; CAPRANICA, R.R. Sexual differences in the auditory system of the tree frog

Eleutherodactylus coqui. Science, v. 192, p. 378–80, 1976.

______. Communicative significance of the two-note call of the treefrog Eleutherodactylus

coqui. J. comp. Physiol., v. 127, p. 1-9, 1978.

OWEN, P.C.; GORDON, N.M. The effect of perceived intruder proximity and resident body size

on the aggressive responses of male green frogs, Rana clamitans (Anura: Ranidae). Behav.

Ecol. Sociobiol., v. 58, p. 446-455, 2005.

PARKER, G.A. Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behavior. J. theor. Biol., v. 47,

(47)

PARRIS, K.M. More bang for your buck: the effect of caller position, habitat and chorus noise on

the efficiency of calling in the spring peeper. Ecological Modelling, v. 156, p. 213-224, 2002.

POOLE, K.L.; MURPHY, C.G. Preferences of female barking treefrogs, Hyla gratiosa, for larger

males: univariate and composite tests. Anim. Behav., v. 73, n. 3, 2007.

R DEVELOPMENT CORE TEAM. R: A language and environment for statistical computing.

Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2010. Disponível em:

<http://www.R-project.org/>.

RAMER, J.D.; JENSSEN, T.A.; HURST, C.J. Size-related variation in the advertisement call of

Rana clamitans (Anura: Ranidae), and its effect on conspecific males. Copeia, v. 1983, n. 1, p.

141-155, 1983.

RASBAND, W.S. ImageJ. Bethesda: U.S. National Institutes of Health, 1997-2009. Disponível

em: <http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij/>.

RICHARDSON, C. LENGAGNE, T. Multiple signals and male spacing affect female preference

at cocktail parties in treefrogs. Proc. R. Soc. B, v. online, p 1-6, 2009. doi:

10.1098/rspb.2009.1836. Acesso em: 02 mar. 2011.

ROBERTSON, J.G.M. Acoustic spacing by breeding males of Uperoleia rugosa (Anura:

Leptodactylidae). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, v. 64, p. 283-297, 1984.

______. Male territoriality, fighting and assessment of fighting ability in the Australian frog

Uperoleia rugosa. Anim. Behav., v. 34, n. 3, p. 763-772, 1986.

RODRIGUES, R.R.; BONONI, V.L.R. (Orgs.) Diretrizes para conservação e restauração da

biodiversidade no Estado de São Paulo. São Paulo: Instituto de Botânica, 2008. 248p.

ROSE, G.J.; BRENOWITZ, E.A. Aggressive thresholds of male Pacific treefrogs for

advertisement call vary with amplitude of neighbors’ calls. Ethology, v. 89, p. 244-252, 1991.

ROSSA-FERES, D.C.; JIM, J. Similaridade no sítio de vocalização em uma comunidade de

anfíbios anuros na região noroeste do Estado de São Paulo, Brasil. Rev. Bras. Zool., v. 18, n.

(48)

RYAN, M.J. Constraints and patterns in the evolution of anuran acoustic communication. In:

FRITZCH, B. (Ed.). The evolution of amphibian auditory system. Nova York: John Wiley &

Sons, Inc, 1988. p. 637-677.

______. Energy, calling and selection. Amer. Zool., v. 28, p. 885-898. 1988.

SÃO PAULO (Estado). Atlas das unidades de conservação ambiental do estado de São

Paulo. São Paulo: Secretaria do Meio Ambiente, 2000.

SCHWARTZ, J.J. The function of call alternation in anuran amphibians: a test of three

hypotheses. Evolution, v. 41, n. 3, p. 461-471, 1987.

______. Male advertisement and female choice in frogs: recent findings and new approaches to

the study of communication in a dynamic acoustic environment. Amer. Zool., v. 34, p. 616-624.

1994.

______. Call monitoring and interactive playback systems in the study of acoustic interactions

among male anurans. In: RYAN, M.J. (Ed.). Anuran communication. Washington: Smithsonian

Institution Press, 2001. p. 183-204.

SCHWARTZ, J.J.; BUCHANAN, R.W.; GERHARDT, H.C. Acoustic interactions among male

gray treefrogs, Hyla versicolor, in a chorus setting. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., v. 53, p. 9-19,

2002.

SHEPARD, D.B. Spatial relationships of male green frogs (Rana clamitans) throughout the

activity season. Am. Midl. Nat., v. 148, n. 2, p. 394–400. 2002.

STEWART, M.M.; BISHOP, P.J. Effects of increased sound level of advertisement calls on

calling male frogs, Eleutherodactylus coqui. J. Herpetol., v. 28, n. 1, p. 46-53, 1994.

TÁRANO, Z.; FUENMAYOR, E. Analysis of the vocalizations of Johnstone's whistling frog

(Eleutherodactylus johnstonei: Eleutherodactylidae) in Northern-Central Venezuela. South

American Journal of Herpetology, v. 3, n. 3, p. 229-238, 2008.

TELFORD, S.R. Mechanisms and evolution of inter-male spacing in the painted reedfrog

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Neste trabalho o objetivo central foi a ampliação e adequação do procedimento e programa computacional baseado no programa comercial MSC.PATRAN, para a geração automática de modelos

Ousasse apontar algumas hipóteses para a solução desse problema público a partir do exposto dos autores usados como base para fundamentação teórica, da análise dos dados

Bom na maioria das vezes, tipo assim, muitos só se importavam com eles, eu ando bem o resto que se lixe, então, muitas vezes tinha que dar um jeito para sobreviver...., bem

Peça de mão de alta rotação pneumática com sistema Push Button (botão para remoção de broca), podendo apresentar passagem dupla de ar e acoplamento para engate rápido

As inscrições para o “Programa de Férias de Verão 2016” decorrem de 4 a 13 de maio de 2016 e poderão ser efetuadas na CAF e na sede administrativa, dentro dos horários

Na hepatite B, as enzimas hepáticas têm valores menores tanto para quem toma quanto para os que não tomam café comparados ao vírus C, porém os dados foram estatisticamente

É nesta mudança, abruptamente solicitada e muitas das vezes legislada, que nos vão impondo, neste contexto de sociedades sem emprego; a ordem para a flexibilização como

Afinal, se o marido, por qualquer circunstância, não puder assum ir a direção da Família, a lei reconhece à mulher aptidão para ficar com os poderes de chefia, substituição que