• Nenhum resultado encontrado

Antidepressivos modificam a extinção de uma memória aversiva em ratas

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2017

Share "Antidepressivos modificam a extinção de uma memória aversiva em ratas"

Copied!
95
0
0

Texto

(1)

1 THIEZA GRAZIELLA ARAUJO DA SILVA GOES DE MELO

ANTIDEPRESSIVOS MODIFICAM A EXTINÇÃO DE UMA MEMÓRIA AVERSIVA EM RATAS

Dissertação apresentada à Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, para obtenção do título de Mestre em Psicobiologia.

(2)

2 THIEZA GRAZIELLA ARAUJO DA SILVA GOES DE MELO

ANTIDEPRESSIVOS MODIFICAM A EXTINÇÃO DE UMA MEMÓRIA AVERSIVA EM RATAS

Dissertação apresentada à Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, para obtenção do título de Mestre em Psicobiologia.

Orientadora: Profª. Drª. Regina Helena da Silva Co-Orientador: MSc PhD Geison de Souza Izídio

(3)

3 Título: ANTIDEPRESSIVOS MODIFICAM A EXTINÇÃO DE UMA MEMÓRIA AVERSIVA EM RATAS

Autor: Thieza Graziella Araújo da Silva Góes de Melo

Data da defesa: 16/05/2011

Banca Examinadora:

___________________________________ Profª. Drª. Flávia Teixeira da Silva Universidade Federal de Sergipe, SE

___________________________________ Profª. Drª. Elaine Cristina Gavioli

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, RN

___________________________________ Profª. Drª. Regina Helena da Silva

(4)

4 AGRADECIMENTOS

À minha família e amigos, pela compreensão. Especialmente minha mãe, por estar sempre presente e ter sempre me dado o melhor exemplo a ser seguido. As minhas conquistas são tão minhas quanto dela e, algumas vezes, talvez até mais dela que minhas. Ao meu noivo, Tiago, também sempre presente, inclusive nos finais de semana, em experimento, me dando suporte. Ao meu padrasto, Pai-Tio Guilherme que tem, com toda certeza, ―direito‖ a pelo menos 1 cm² do certificado. À minha avó, meu exemplo de vida, e que me mostrou que tudo pode ser feito com paciência, boa vontade e um pouquinho de ousadia.

À professora Regina que me acompanha desde a iniciação científica, dando suporte acadêmico/científico e que verdadeiramente me ensinou a fazer ciência, mesmo sob as circunstâncias mais adversas.

Ao LEME, no corpo de seus ICs, mestrandos, doutorandos, pós-docs e professores pela ajuda durante os experimentos e dos quais recebi não menos que amizade. Dentro do LEME agradeço especialmente às duas pessoas que participaram mais diretamente nos experimentos e revisões, Luane e Geison.

Aos dois psiquiatras que me forneceram suporte técnico e informações clínicas Drª Nádira Hazboun e Drº Francisco Márcio Pinheiro.

À UFRN, CAPES, CNPq, FAPERN e PPG em Psicobiologia pelo apoio financeiro e estrutural.

A todos os professores e colegas do programa de pós-graduação em Psicobiologia.

(5)

5 Resumo

Os tratamentos de depressão, transtorno de estresse pós-traumático e outras psicopatologias que se utilizam de antidepressivos podem estar associados à melhora de déficits cognitivos relacionados a esses transtornos. Embora os mecanismos pelos quais a melhora nos déficits cognitivos ocorre não estejam totalmente esclarecidos, alterações na extinção de memórias aversivas podem estar presentes nestas psicopatologias. Além disso, pesquisas com animais de laboratório geralmente são realizadas com indivíduos do sexo masculino, e recentemente verificamos que a extinção de uma tarefa aversiva é diminuída em ratas quando comparada ao desempenho de ratos. No presente estudo, ratas Wistar foram tratadas prolongadamente com antidepressivos utilizados na clínica (nortriptilina, fluoxetina ou mirtazapina) e testadas na esquiva discriminativa em labirinto em cruz elevado e no teste do nado forçado, a fim de avaliar a aprendizagem, a memória, a extinção, a ansiedade e comportamentos relacionados à depressão. A exploração do braço aversivo na sessão de treino foi semelhante em todos os grupos, mostrando que todos os grupos aprenderam a tarefa, havendo, porém, uma melhora no desempenho dos grupos tratados com nortriptilina e mirtazapina. Na sessão teste todos os animais evocaram a tarefa. O tratamento prolongado com a fluoxetina, mas não com os outros antidepressivos, promoveu uma melhora na extinção da memória aversiva da EDLC. No teste de nado forçado, os animais tratados com fluoxetina e mirtazapina apresentaram diminuição na duração da imobilidade, comparados ao veículo. Em conclusão, os antidepressivos podem interferir no aprendizado, mas não na evocação de memórias aversivas. Além disso, ratas tratadas com fluoxetina apresentam um aumento da extinção da tarefa aversiva, em comparação ao veículo, enquanto os demais tratamentos impediram a extinção dessa tarefa. Além disso, tanto a fluoxetina como a mirtazapina foram eficazes no teste de nado forçado, sugerindo dissociação entre os efeitos antidepressivos e extinção de memórias aversivas.

(6)

6 Abstract

Treatment of major depression, posttraumatic stress disorder and other psychopathologies with antidepressants can be associated with improvement of the cognitive deficits related to these disorders. Although the mechanisms of these effects are not completely elucidated, alterations in extinction of aversive memories are believed to be present in these psychopathologies. Moreover, researches with laboratory animals usually focus on male subjects, and we have recently verified that extinction of an aversive task is reduced in female rats when compared to males. In the present study, female rats were long-term treated with clinically used antidepressants (fluoxetine, nortriptyline or mirtazapine) and tested in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance and forced swimming tests in order to evaluate learning, memory, extinction, anxiety and depression-related behaviors. All groups learned the task, but learning was somewhat faster in nortriptyline and mirtazapine-treated animals . Task retrieval was also showed by all experimental groups. Chronic treatment with fluoxetine, but not with the other antidepressants, increased extinction of the discriminative task. In the forced swimming test, animals treated with fluoxetine and mirtazapine showed decreased immobility duration. In conclusion, antidepressants interfere with learning and female rats treated with fluoxetine presented increased extinction of the aversive memory task. On the other hand, both fluoxetine and mirtazapine were effective in the forced swimming test, suggesting dissociation between the antidepressant effects and the extinction of aversive memories.

(7)

7 Sumário

I. INTRODUÇÃO ... 8

II. OBJETIVOS ... 20

III. ARTIGO PARA SUBMISSÃO... 21

IV. CONCLUSÃO E CONSIDERAÇÕES FINAIS... 55

(8)

8 I. Introdução

O uso de antidepressivos, sozinhos ou em combinação com a psicoterapia, é largamente difundido. Além da grande indicação para o tratamento de psicopatologias como transtornos de humor e ansiedade (Marks et al. 1998; Brunello et al. 2001; Nandam et al. 2007), eles são também indicados como uma opção alternativa para doenças debilitantes funcionais como, por exemplo, a síndrome da fadiga crônica, a fibromialgia, enxaquecas ou dores de cabeça crônicas e dores faciais atípicas (Marks et al. 2008; Mao et al. 2010).

A depressão é uma psicopatologia já devidamente classificada no Manual de Diagnóstico e Estatística de Doenças Mentais (em inglês Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders), com suas últimas modificações na classificação para

(9)

9 psicomotora; diminuição da energia; sentimentos de desvalia ou culpa; dificuldades para pensar, concentrar-se ou tomar decisões, ou pensamentos recorrentes sobre morte ou ideação suicida; planos ou tentativas de suicídio. Sintomas físicos são comumente encontrados no episódio depressivo maior: propensão ao choro, irritabilidade, ruminação obsessiva, ansiedade, fobias, preocupação com a saúde física e queixas de dores, tanto de cabeça, quanto no corpo (APA 2000; Fava & Kendler 2000). Alguns indivíduos podem apresentar ataques de pânico que ocorrem segundo um padrão que satisfaz os critérios para transtorno de pânico. Em crianças pode ocorrer ansiedade de separação. Alguns indivíduos observam dificuldade nos relacionamentos íntimos, diminuição das interações sociais satisfatórias ou dificuldades no funcionamento sexual (APA 2000). A manifestação de episódios depressivos pode ainda vir como um transtorno depressivo maior, o qual é caracterizado pela ocorrência de um ou mais episódios depressivos, sem história de episódios maníacos, mistos ou hipomaníacos.

(10)

10 uso de antidepressivos também ao tratamento de outros transtornos como os da ansiedade (APA 2000; Marques 2001).

A incidência desses demais transtornos também pode ser relacionada à comorbidade deles com a depressão. Os transtornos de ansiedade mais comuns em mulheres, em sua grande maioria (Bekker & van Mens-Verhulst 2007) são os de maiores índices de comorbidade com a depressão. Dessa forma é imperativa a necessidade de uma maior atenção à sintomatologia diferenciada apresentada pelos indivíduos do sexo feminino. Mulheres e crianças podem apresentar mais comumente o humor irritado (APA 2000) e essa alteração no humor pode ser sintoma apenas da depressão e não ter nenhuma relação com outros transtornos de humor ou ansiedade. Paralelamente, a perda de peso pode estar associada mais facilmente aos transtornos alimentares, que podem ser a causa da depressão (APA 2000; Mischoulon et al. 2010; Modrzejewska 2010).

O tratamento de transtornos do humor é geralmente complexo, prolongado e adequado a cada indivíduo. Estudos com indivíduos saudáveis sugerem que a percepção de faces com expressões negativas começam a aparecer após o 7º dia de tratamento (Harmer et al. 2010), diminuição de perceptção essa, associada aos efeitos terapêuticos do uso contínuo de antidepressivos. Existem várias classes de drogas antidepressivas disponíveis na atualidade, com propriedades farmacodinâmicas distintas. De forma geral, tais fármacos aumentam as concentrações de monoaminas na fenda sináptica, mas apenas esse aumento, embora necessário, não é suficiente para explicar a remissão dos sintomas (Arantes-Gonçalves & Coelho 2006).

(11)

11 noradrenalina (NA) na fenda sináptica, visto que drogas que depletam as reservas desses neurotransmissores são indutoras de quadros depressivos (Freis 1954; Guay 2010; Tian et al 2010; Aia et al 2011; Ghia et al 2011), e o mecanismo de ação dos agentes depressivos mais utilizados envolve aumento da disponibilidade desses neurotransmissores (Kent 2000). Contudo, uma hipótese mais recente sobre a fisiopatologia e tratamento da depressão envolve também adaptação e plasticidade neural. De acordo com essa hipótese, a depressão seria resultado de uma falha na execução de respostas adaptativas apropriadas ao estresse e a estímulos aversivos, ou seja, perda da plasticidade sináptica. A isso pode se associar o fato de que a exposição crônica a situações de estresse induz um quadro semelhante à depressão, com redução dos níveis hipocampais de fator neurotrófico derivado do encéfalo (BDNF – do inglês brain-derived neutrophic factor) (Gould et al. 1999; Sen et al. 2003; Arantes-Gonçalves & Coelho 2006), componente da família das neurotrofinas, que, entre várias outras moléculas, apresenta um papel reconhecidamente importante na plasticidade sináptica (Arantes-Gonçalves & Coelho 2006; Monfils 2007; Kalueff 2007).

(12)

12 Dentre as várias opções de antidepressivos os mais prescritos são os inibidores seletivos de recaptação de serotonina (ISRS) tanto para adultos como para crianças (Dunlop & Davis 2008; Denizot et al. 2009). Introduzidos na clínica no final da década de 80 (Kent 2000), os ISRS agem bloqueando a ação do transportador de serotonina. Dessa forma eles impedem a recaptação do neurotransmissor, o que prolonga a sua exposição ao receptor, (Vázquez-Palacios 2004) e aumentando a atividade do sistema monoaminérgico no cérebro (Tsai et al. 2009). Dentre os antidepressivos dessa classe a fluoxetina é o mais prescrito (Dunlop & Davis 2008). É importante ressaltar que a serotonina (5-HT) possui também um papel regulador na divisão celular e um papel crítico no controle da proliferação das células adultas neurais, fato que explicaria sua eficácia na melhora dos sintomas depressivos considerando o papel da neurogênese hipocampal na fisiopatologia da depressão (Santarelli et al. 2003; Paizanis et al. 2007)

(13)

13 colateral importante decorrente do tratamento com ISRS (Dagtekin et al. 2010; Diaz & Maroteaux 2011).

Os antidepressivos tetracíclicos são uma nova classe de drogas utilizadas no tratamento da depressão, também chamados de atípicos, introduzidos depois da década de 80 (Kent 2000). Dentre estes, o antidepressivo atípico mirtazapina atua especificamente sobre as transmissões noradrenérgica e serotonérgica (noradrenergic and serotonergic specific antidepressants, NaSSA, em inglês) e é usado para tratar transtornos de humor e ansiedade (Gambi et al. 2005; Rauggi et al. 2005). A mirtazapina aumenta as transmissões noradrenérgica e serotonérgica central através da inibição dos autoreceptores α2 e heteroreceptores noradrenérgicos α2 em sinapses serotonérgicas (Bengtsson et al. 2000; Gambi et al. 2005). Além disso, é um agonista do receptor 5-HT1A (Rogóz et al. 2005) e também age bloqueando receptores, 5-HT2A, 5-HT2C e 5-HT3 e histaminérgicos H1 (Haddjeri 1998; Davis & Wilde 1996; Arnone 2009; de Boer 1995; Rauggi et al. 2005). A mirtazapina não possui efeitos na recaptação de monoaminas, além de apresentar baixa afinidade para receptores dopaminérgicos e alguns subtipos de receptores serotonérgicos.

(14)

14 tricíclico, enquanto machos respondem positivamente ao mesmo tratamento e com a mesma dose (Barros & Ferigolo 1998).

Assim como em grande parte dos casos de diversos transtornos psicopatológicos, pacientes com diagnóstico de depressão, não raramente apresentam prejuízos cognitivos, além de todo quadro afetivo característico (Yaffe 1999; Ravnkilde 2002; Pardo 2006). Dentre esses prejuízos incluem-se alterações nas funções de memória e aprendizado (Burt 1995; Weiland-Fiedler 2004). Esses prejuízos cognitivos seriam revertidos através do tratamento com antidepressivos (Austin et al. 2001; Castaneda et al. 2007) e evidências sugerem que essa melhora está associada ao aumento de neurogênese (Duman & Monteggia 2006; Dranovsky & Hen 2006; Nandam et al. 2007; Paizanis et al. 2007; Sahay & Hen 2007; Pittenger & Duman 2008) e a modificações no sistema monoaminérgico (Lee et al. 2010).

(15)

15 al. 2006; Krystal & Neumeister 2009), a participação de processos relacionados à memória emocional e a fisiopatologia da depressão não está bem estabelecida (Blaney 1986; Newman & Sweet 1992; Ilsley et al. 1995; Veiel 1997; Fava & Kendler 2000; Ravnkilde 2002). Apesar de prejuízos na memória serem comuns em diversos transtornos de ansiedade e relatados na depressão, a conexão entre antidepressivos e memória permanece ainda não completamente elucidada (Austin et al. 2001). Os resultados dos estudos prévios são bastante controversos, mesmo em machos. Estudos que avaliam a extinção de memórias aversivas após a administração de drogas com efeito antidepressivo mostram resultados contraditórios. Dentre estes estudos, alguns, utilizando machos, verificam os efeitos do rolipram, uma droga com efeitos ansiolíticos e antidepressivos. Essa droga diminuiu a extinção em um teste de medo condicionado e em um paradigma de resposta de sobressalto potencializada pelo medo (Monti et al. 2006; Mueller et al., 2010). Além disso, este fármaco melhorou o desempenho no labirinto aquático de Morris, aumentando a distância e o tempo em que os animais nadam no quadrante no qual previamente havia uma plataforma submersa; e ainda impediu a extinção relacionada a uma tarefa de esquiva passiva (Cheng et al, 2010). Um outro estudo foi realizado com um modelo animal para transtorno de estresse pós-traumático, característico pela ausência de extinção da memória do evento traumático (APA 2000). Nesse estudo, a cicloserina-D (DCS), droga utilizada como auxiliar em tratamentos antidepressivos, melhorou a extinção em testes de condicionamento de medo em machos (Yamamoto et al. 2008).

(16)

16 pelas doenças debilitantes funcionais e em grande parte dos transtornos psicopatológicos a proporção dos indivíduos atingidos é de duas mulheres para cada homem (Mahedran & Yap 2005; Bekker & van Mens-Verhulst 2007). Apesar de não se descartar fatores culturais e sociais, diversas evidências indicam que a principal origem dessas diferenças está relacionada aos hormônios esteróides gonadais (Toufexis et al. 2006; Solomon & Herman 2009), que podem ter efeitos nas vias neuroquímicas (Kelly et al. 1999). Além disso, aspectos do dimorfismo sexual no funcionamento do sistema nervoso também podem estar relacionados a estas diferenças (Bruder et al. 2001; Esel et al. 2005; Monteggia et al 2007; Elaković et al. 2011).

(17)

17 pode ter relação com a predominância de psicopatologias em seres humanos do sexo feminino.

No presente estudo, nós utilizamos o modelo utilizado por Ribeiro et al. (2010): a esquiva discriminativa em labirinto em cruz elevado (EDLC). A EDLC é um método adequado para estudos concomitantes de memória/aprendizado e medo/ansiedade em modelos animais. Consiste em um labirinto em cruz elevado modificado, onde um dos braços fechados possui dois estímulos aversivos (sendo um sonoro de intensidade equivalente a 80dB e um luminoso de 100W). Os animais são submetidos a uma sessão de treino, na qual uma vez colocados individualmente no centro do labirinto por 10 min, poderão explorar os braços, recebendo os estímulos aversivos cada vez que o animal entrar com as quatro patas no braço aversivo, até a saída do braço em questão. O tempo de permanência e o total de entradas nos braços abertos e em cada braço fechado são quantificados. Decorridas 24 horas da sessão de treino é efetuada uma sessão de teste, na qual não se aplica o estímulo aversivo. Dessa forma, ao longo da sessão pode-se também avaliar a extinção da tarefa previamente aprendida, uma vez que os animais agora aprendem que não há mais estímulos aversivos naquela localização. Este modelo foi inicialmente descrito por Silva (1997) e Silva & Frussa-Filho (2000), que padronizaram o modelo com machos e averiguaram que, usualmente os três minutos iniciais são referentes à evocação da memória adquirida e os demais à extinção.

(18)

18 de ansiedade a exploração desses braços poderá aumentar ou diminuir. Considerando que alterações nos níveis de ansiedade podem afetar o perfil comportamental dos animais em modelos animais de estudo de memória, o modelo da EDLC apresenta a grande vantagem de permitir a avaliação simultânea destes dois parâmetros (Calzavara et al. 2004; Silva & Frussa-Filho 2000). A diferenciação entre os braços fechados (aversivo e não-aversivo) serve como parâmetro para avaliação de memória e aprendizado. Esse modelo permite ainda a avaliação da atividade locomotora que pode ser realizada por meio da exploração de todos os braços e do centro, pelo total de passagens entre os braços (Silva et al. 2002) ou, ainda, quantificando-se a distância total percorrida (Ribeiro et al., 2010).

(19)

19 tentativa de fuga de situações aversivas, entre machos e fêmeas (Ribeiro et al. 2010), embora em outro tipo de tarefa comportamental. A diminuição da duração da imobilidade e o aumento do tempo de escalada podem estar relacionados a uma melhora dos sintomas tipo depressivos através da transmissão noradrenérgica (Detke et al. 1995, 1997; Reneric & Lucki 1998; Page et al. 1999; Cryan & Lucki 2000; Lopez-Rubalcava & Lucki 2000; Cryan et al. 2002a; Reneric et al. 2002; Consoni et al. 2006).

(20)

20 II. Objetivos

Objetivo Geral

O presente estudo propôs-se a averiguar os efeitos de três antidepressivos provenientes de classes farmacológicas distintas no aprendizado, memória, extinção, ansiedade/emocionalidade e comportamento de desamparo aprendido em ratas. Tendo em vista a prevalência dos transtornos relacionados à depressão e ansiedade em mulheres e a pequena quantidade de pesquisas relacionando os efeitos desses fármacos em processos cognitivos em indivíduos do sexo feminino, a nossa proposta pretende contribuir no avanço da compreensão dos efeitos destas classes de drogas em fêmeas.

Objetivos específicos

(a) Avaliar as diferentes etapas de memória (aquisição, consolidação, evocação e extinção) em fêmeas tratadas com os antidepressivos na EDLC;

(b) Avaliar os comportamentos relacionados à ansiedade em fêmeas tratadas com os antidepressivos na EDLC;

(c) Avaliar possíveis efeitos locomotores dos fármacos utilizados;

(21)

21 III. Artigo para submissão

Date of submission:

ANTIDEPRESSANTS FLUOXETINE, NORTRIPTYLINE AND MIRTAZAPINE MODIFIES THE EXTINCTION OF AN AVERSIVE MEMORY AND THE ANXIETY IN

FEMALE RATS

Thieza G. Melo, Geison S. Izídio, Luane S. Ferreira, Diego S. Silveira, Priscila T. Macedo, Alícia Cabral, Alessandra M. Ribeiro, Regina H. Silva

Memory Studies Laboratory Department of Physiology

Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte 59.078-970, Natal, RN, Brazil·.

*Corresponding author: Regina H. Silva Laboratório de Estudos da Memória

Departamento de Fisiologia - Centro de Biociências Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte

Av. Salgado Filho, s/n - Caixa Postal 1511 - CEP 59078-970 - Natal, RN, Brazil fax: (55) 84 3211 9206 e-mail: reginahsilva@gmail.com

Progress In Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry (Classificação Qualis A2, em Fevereiro de 2011)

(22)

22 Abstract

(23)

23 1. Introduction

Psychopathological conditions such mood and anxiety disorders are frequently related to aversive emotional experiences (APA, 2000) that could benefit from a reframing of the aversive memory formatted, which is usually attempted in psychotherapeutic procedures (Freud, 1914). It has also been suggested that the cognitive deficits present in these disorders are reversed by treatment with antidepressants (Austin et al., 2001; Castaneda et al., 2007), which are largely indicated for both anxiety and mood disorders (Marks et al., 1998; Brunello et al., 2001; Nandam et al., 2007). Several studies indicate that the improvement of these cognitive deficits is associated with hippocampal neurogenesis (Santarelli et al., 2003; Duman and Monteggia, 2006; Dranovsky and Hen, 2006; Nandam et al., 2007; Paizanis et al., 2007; Sahay and Hen, 2007; Pittenger and Duman, 2008), which would explain the delay of amelioration of depressive symptoms after the beginning of clinical treatment. Despite memory deficits being common in many anxiety and depression- related disorders the relationship between the improvement of depression or anxiety symptoms by antidepressants treatment, as well as their effect on memory, remains to be elucidated (Austin et al., 2001).

(24)
(25)

25 In the present study, we investigated the effects of three antidepressants (fluoxetine, nortriptyline and mirtazapine) of different pharmacological classes on learning, memory and extinction of an aversive task in female rats, as well as anxiety and depression-related behaviors in the same subjects. Fluoxetine is a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), the most prescribed class of antidepressants (Dunlop and Davis, 2008). Nortriptyline is a tricyclic antidepressant, an older class of drugs, and act by nonselective inhibition of monoaminergic reuptake. Although these compounds are no longer largely prescribed, they are still an option to individuals which are non-responsive to other antidepressants and to subjects susceptible to the serotonin syndrome (Dagtekin et al., 2010). Finally, the atypical antidepressant mirtazapine is used to treat both mood and anxiety disorders (Rauggi et al., 2005; Gambi et al., 2005) and enhances central noradrenergic and serotonergic neurotransmission through inhibition of the noradrenergic α2-autoreceptor and α2 -heteroreceptor in serotonergic synapses (Bengtsson et al., 2000; Gambi et al., 2005).

We used the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task (PMDAT) to evaluate concomitantly memory and anxiety-related behaviors. Several studies performed with this task have shown the effects of memory-enhancing or amnestic drugs, procedures that modify anxiety-like behaviors and/or locomotor activity (Silva et al., 1997; Silva and Frussa-Filho, 2000; Silva et al., 2002a; Silva et al., 2002b). It also fits for the evaluation of learning and extinction processes, by the evaluation of aversive arm avoidance across the behavioral sessions (Silva et al., 2004; Ribeiro et al., 2010; see Methods). Additionally, female rats were also tested in the forced swimming test (FST) in order to evaluate depression-related behaviors.

(26)

26 2.1 Animals

Three-month-old female Wistar rats (120–230g), from our own colony were housed in groups of 4 animals in plastic cages (30 x 37 x 16 cm) in a room with acoustic isolation, airflow and controlled conditions of temperature (24 – 26 °C), humidity and luminosity (12h light: 12h dark, lights on 06h30). Food and water were available ad libitum throughout the experiments. Animal care was according to Brazilian law nº 11.794/2008 for the use of animals in research, and all experiments were approved by the local ethical committee (CEUA-UFRN). All efforts were made to minimize animal pain, suffering or discomfort as well as the number of animals used.

2.2 Procedures and drugs

(27)

27 predominance of any stage among the experimental groups during the experiments (data not shown). Rats were handled for the whole injection period, for 5 minutes per day. From the 17th day of treatment onwards, behavioral tests started at 1:30 p.m. and injections and cycle control continued at 6 p.m., after the behavioral procedure.

2.3 Plus-maze discriminative avoidance task (PMDAT)

(28)

28 (Rodgers et al., 1997). A decrease in risk assessment behavior and an increase in head dipping indicate less anxious behavior (Rodgers and Dalvi, 1997; Rodgers et al., 1997). Distance traveled in the apparatus, percent time spent in the aversive enclosed arm (time spent in aversive enclosed arm / time spent in both enclosed arms) and percent time spent in open arms (time spent in open arms / time spent in both open and enclosed arms) were registered min by min throughout the sessions and used to evaluate motor activity, learning/memory/extinction and anxiety, respectively. The behavior of the animals was monitored and analyzed by the video-tracking software ANY-maze, Stoelting, USA.

2.4 Forced Swimming Test (FST)

For evaluation of the depression-related behaviors we used the FST (Porsolt et al., 1977). This test consists in placing the animal in a cylinder (40 cm high, 25 cm diameter) with water (30 cm deep) in a temperature of 24 to 27°C, for two consecutive days. On the first day, animals were submitted to 15 min of forced exposure (pre-test session) with no behavioral observation. In the second day, 24 h after the pre-test session, rats were placed once again in the water tank (test session) in the same conditions described above and total immobility duration, climbing behavior and the latency to engage in immobility were registered for 5 min.

2.5 Statistical Analyses

(29)

29 total length of the sessions in the PMDAT, behavioral sessions were divided in 5 blocks of 2 minutes each. Comparison among these blocks was used to evaluate learning (training) or extinction (test) of the task. ANOVAs with repeated measures were used to compare the percent time spent in the aversive and open arm throughout PMDAT sessions and pairwise comparisons was used. We used the software SPSS (version 17) to perform the statistical analysis. Differences were considered significant at p<0.05.

3. Results

Plus-maze discriminative avoidance task:

(30)

30 the aversive arm during the first session block (paired samples t-test) (Fig. 1B). The ANOVA for the 2 minutes session blocks showed no difference between the groups for each session (for the first block [F (3,34) = 0.58; p = 0.62], second [F (3,34) = 1.00; p = 0.40], third [F (3,34) = 1.04; p = 0.38], fourth [F (3,34) = 1.30; p = 0.29] and fifth [F (3,34) = 2.03; p = 0.13]).

Comparison of the exploration of the aversive arm was marginally significant in the test session [F (3,34) = 2.52; p = 0.07], and pos hoc analysis with Bonferroni‘s

(31)

31 Percentage of time spent in the open arms did not differ between different treatments, suggesting that animals did not present anxiety-related differences [training session: F (3,34) = 0.060; p = 0.98; test session: F (3,34) = 0.50; p = 0.68] (Fig. 2). Similarly, no differences were found in the frequency of head dipping [training session: F (3,34) = 1.352; p = 0.276; test session: F (3,34) = 0.060; p = 0.98] (Fig.3C and D). A treatment effect was found for time spent in risk assessment in the test [F (3,34) = 3.876; p = 0.018] (Fig. 3B), but not in the training session [F (3,34) = 0.278; p = 0.841] (Fig. 3A). In the test session, the post hoc with Bonferroni‘s test

showed a significant difference between vehicle and fluoxetine (p = 0.04), while the comparison between vehicle and nortriptyline almost reached significance (p = 0.05).

When the whole sessions were considered for analysis, ANOVA revealed a treatment effect in the distance traveled in the apparatus [F (3,34) = 4.56; p < 0.01] and [F (3,34) = 3.86; p < 0.05] in training and test, respectively. Post hoc with Bonferroni‘s test showed that animals treated with mirtazapine exhibited a

significantly decreased locomotor activity compared to the other groups (Table 1). In the forced swimming test, ANOVA revealed a treatment effect in immobility duration [F (3,33) = 13.64; p < 0.001]. Post hoc with Bonferroni‘s test showed that

animals treated with fluoxetine and mirtazapine had decreased immobility duration compared with the control and nortriptyline groups (Fig. 4A). The latency to start immobility was not different among the groups (data not shown). Moreover, ANOVA revealed a treatment effect on climbing behavior [F (3,33) = 4.17; p = 0.014]. Post hoc with Bonferroni‘s test showed that fluoxetine group had an increased time in

(32)
(33)
(34)
(35)
(36)
(37)
(38)

38 Table 1. Distance travelled in meters in both training and test session by female rats repeatedly treated with vehicle, 20mg/kg fluoxetine, 20 mg/kg nortriptyline or 10 mg/kg mirtazapine; *p<0.05 compared to all other groups (ANOVA followed by Bonferroni‘s test).

4. Discussion

In summary, our data showed that some antidepressants could interfere on both learning and memory processes, more specifically on the acquisition and, more importantly, extinction of an aversive task. The nortriptyline- and mirtazapine-treated groups learned the task faster than the vehicle and fluoxetine counterparts, as demonstrated by a significant decrease in the aversive arm exploration earlier in the training session (see figure 1B). During the test session, vehicle and fluoxetine groups have shown extinction of the task, i.e., significant increases in aversive arm exploration across the session blocks, what did not occur in the other groups (see figure 1D). Moreover, the extinction was anticipated by treatment with fluoxetine. The treatment with fluoxetine and nortriptyline also reduced one of the anxiety-related behaviors in the elevated plus-maze discriminative avoidance task (risk assessment behavior, see figure 3B). Results obtained in the forced swimming test showed that

Treatment Training Test

Vehicle 19,0±2,4 21,1±1,9

Fluoxetine 20,0±3,0 21,6±4,4

Nortriptyline 22,9±3,1 16,8±4,4

(39)

39 treatment with fluoxetine and mirtazapine reduced the immobility time, which is consistent with the antidepressant-like effects of these drugs (see figure 4A).

As mentioned above, the analysis of the aversive arm exploration throughout the test session of the PMDAT provides indication of both task retrieval (beginning of session) and extinction (subsequent session blocks) (Ribeiro et al., 2010). Indeed, with the aversive stimuli no longer present, the animals avoid the aversive arm at first, but eventually realize the arm is now safe and increase its exploration, reaching the amount they would explore a regular enclosed arm by chance (or even more, since this arm is almost novel for them in the test session). This pattern is usually observed in males, but in a previous study we have shown that female rats keep avoiding the aversive arm until the end of the test, or even at a subsequent retest (Ribeiro et al., 2010). The lack of extinction by females points out to stronger consolidation and/or impaired extinction of aversive memories, which is in line with the better emotional memory usually reported for women (Canli et al., 2002; Hamann, 2005). In particularly, this lack of extinction is similar to what occur in some anxiety disorders, as for example, the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and it could be related to the greater prevalence of depression and anxiety disorders among female gender (Kendler et al., 2001; Mahedran, 2005; Rauch et al., 2006; Bekker and van Mens-Verhulst, 2007).

(40)

40 raise the hypothesis that fluoxetine could exert its therapeutic action by modulating the extinction of aversive memories. In line with this reasoning, this drug has been shown potential benefits in the treatment of PTSD, which has a close relationship with deficits in extinction of traumatic memories (APA, 2000; Quirk et al., 2006; Yamamoto et al., 2008; 2009). Moreover, despite the effects of fluoxetine treatment on males in the same test has not been investigated yet, these results, taking together with previous extinction studies performed with males (see Introduction) suggest an important role of gender in the ability of antidepressants in modifying extinction.

(41)

41 fluoxetine administered chronically to adult rats reduces, in the amygdala, the polysialylated form of the neural cell adhesion molecule (PSA-NCAM), a molecule involved in the synaptogenesis (Varea et al. 2007; Homberg et al., 2011). It has also been proposed that some types of antidepressants could elicit an increase on the perception of the threat cues and improve the trace of the formatted memory avoiding or decreasing its extinction (Rawlings et al., 2010). In summary, it seems plausible that the clinical action of antidepressants could be related to a modulation of plastic processes related to extinction of aversive memories.

(42)

42 aversive memory formatted could appear (Morgan and LeDoux, 1995; Lebrón et al., 2004; Quirk et al., 2006).

The hypolocomotor effects observed here in females treated with mirtazapine have already been found in other studies in this same dose range (Reneric et al., 2002) or not (Rauggi et al., 2005). It is interesting to note that, in the present study, the hypolocomotor effects were specifically found in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task, i.e. they were not present in the forced swimming test. It is known that mirtazapine enhances both 5-HT and NA neurotransmissions, yet differently from monoamine reuptake inhibitors since its antidepressant effects are mediated through the direct antagonism of both α2 and 5-HT2C receptors (de Boer et al., 1996;

Haddjeri et al., 1996). It has been shown that this mediation can cause hypolocomotor effects (Franowicz et al., 2002). Mirtazapine also induces blockage of histamine-H1 receptors, other possible mechanism that may produce sedation and decrease the exploratory activity (Schüle et al., 2003; 2006; Gambi et al., 2005).

(43)

43 a better evaluation of the parameters. On the other hand, some authors argue that the evaluation of anxiety-related behaviors in a re-exposition to the conventional elevated plus-maze is not adequate, because the reasoning of the paradigm implies novelty (File, 1990).

(44)

44 al. 1999; Mirza et al., 2007) while others showed anxiogenic effects (File et al., 1999; Robert et al., 2011).

The forced swimming test is normally used in the screening of antidepressant drugs, with immobility duration levels being used to measure indices of ―behavioral despair‖ (Borsini and Meli, 1988). Moreover, the immobility presented by the animal

is reversed by repeated antidepressant treatment and for this reason it is used as an index of ‗depressive-like‘ state (Cryan et al., 2005; Porsolt et al., 1978). In the present study, animals treated with fluoxetine and mirtazapine, as expected, showed decreased time of immobility duration in the forced swimming test. However, females treated with nortriptyline did not exhibit this decrease, at a dose that was previously shown to be effective in a study with males (Consoni et al., 2006). They also showed less climbing behavior than fluoxetine–treated animals, suggesting that nortriptyline group were less active in this behavioral test. However, the possibility that these effects were only due to alterations in locomotion seems unlikely, because in the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task there were no significant decreases in ambulation of nortriptyline-treated animals.

(45)
(46)

46 5. References

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2000. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychatric Association.

Andrade, S., Silveira, S.L., Arbo, B.D., Batista, B.A., Gomez, R., Barros, H.M., Ribeiro, M.F. 2010. Sex-dependent antidepressant effects of lower doses of progesterone in rats. Physiol Behav. Apr 19; 99(5):687-90.

Astur, R.S., Tropp, J., Sava, S., Constable, R.T. & Marku, E.J. 2004. Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation. Behavioral Brain Research, 151, 103-115.

Austin, M.-P., Mitchell, P., Goodwin, G.M. 2001. Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology, British Journal of Psychiatry 178, 200-206.

Babbini, M., Gaiardi, M., Bartoletti, M. 1976. Effects of combined treatment with nortriptiline and lorazepam on conflict behavior and motility of rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). Aug 17;48(3):251-4.

Beck, A.T., Rush, A.J., Shaw, B.F., Emery, G. 1979. Cognitive Therapy of Depression. Guilford.

Bengtsson, H.J., Kele, J., Johansson, J. Hjorth, S. 2000. Interaction of the antidepressant mirtazapine with α2-adrenoceptors modulating the release of 5-HT in different rat brain regions in vivo, Naunyn-Schmiedeberg‘s Arch Pharmacol 362 :406–412.

Bekker, M.H.J., Mens-Verhulst, J. van, 2007. Anxiety Disorders: Sex Differences in Prevalence, Degree, and Background, But Gender-Neutral Treatment, Gender Medicine/VoL 4, SUPPL. B, 2007.

Bigos, K.L., Pollock, B.G., Stankevich, B.A., Bies, R.R. 2009. Sex differences in the pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of antidepressants: an updated review. Gend Med. Dec;6(4):522-43. Review. Nature 465 10 June.

Blokland, A., Rutten, K., Prickaerts, J. 2006. Analysis of spatial orientation strategies of male and female Wistar rats in a Morris water escape task. Behav. Brain Res., 171, 216-224.

Borsini, F., Meli, A. 1988. Is the forced swimming test a suitable model for revealing antidepressant activity? Psychopharmacology (Berl);94(2):147-60.

Borsini, F., Podhorna, J., Marazziti, D. 2002. Do animal models of anxiety predict anxiolytic-like effects of antidepressants? Psychopharmacology, 163:121– 141.

(47)

47 reuptake. A pharmacological characterization of diverse classes of antidepressant agents. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. Apr;71(4):667-80.

Brunello, N., Davidson, J.R., Deahl, M., Kessler, R.C., Mendlewicz, J., Racagni, G., Shalev, A.Y., Zohar, J. 2001. Posttraumatic stress disorder: diagnosis and epidemiology, comorbidity and social consequences, biology and treatment. Neuropsychobiology ;43(3):150-62.

Cassol-Jr., O.J., Comim, C.M., Petronilho, F., Constantino, L.S., Streck, E.L., Quevedo, J., Dal-Pizzol, F. 2010. Low dose dexamethasone reverses depressive-like parameters and memory impairment in rats submitted to sepsis. Neuroscience Letters 473; 126–130

Canli, T., Desmond, J., Zhao, A., Gabrielli, J.D.E. 2002. Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories. PNAS, 99(16), 10789-10794.

Castaneda, A.E., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Marttunen, M., Suvisaari, J, Lönnqvist, J. 2007. A review on cognitive impairments in depressive and anxiety disorders with a focus on young adults. J. Affect. Disord.

Castrén, E., Rantamaki, T. 2010. The Role of BDNF and Its Receptors in Depression and Antidepressant Drug Action: Reactivation of Developmental Plasticity. Dev Neurobiol. Apr;70(5):289-97.

Cheng, Y.F., Wang, C., Lin, H.B., Li, Y.F., Huang, Y., Xu, J.P., Zhang, H.T. 2010. Inhibition of phosphodiesterase-4 reverses memory deficits produced by Aβ 25-35 or Aβ1-40 peptide in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl). Oct; 212(2):181-91.

Consoni, F.T., Vital, M.A., Andreatini, R. 2006. Dual monoamine modulation for the antidepressant-like effect of lamotrigine in the modified forced swimming test. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol, v 16(6), p. 451-458.

Contreras, C.M., Martinez-Mota, L., Saavedra, M. 1998. Desipramine restricts estral cycle oscillations in swimming. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, v. 22, p. 1121-1128,

Cryan, J.F., Valentino, R.J., Lucki, I. 2005. Assessing substrates underlying the behavioral effects of antidepressants using the modified rat forced swimming test. Neurosci Biobehav Rev ;29:547–69.

Dagtekin, O., Marcus, H., Müller, C., Böttinger, B.W., Spöhr, F. 2010. Liquid therapy for serotonin syndrome after intoxication with venlafaxine, lamotrigine and diazepam, Minerva Anestesiologica, v. 76.

Dalla, C., Pitychoutis, P.M., Kokras, N., Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z. 2009. Sex Differences in Animal Models of Depression and Antidepressant Response. Basic & Clinical Pharmacology & Toxicology, 106, 226–233.

(48)

48 dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex by long-term treatment with antidepressant drugs. Journal of Neurochemistry, 76, 1212±1220.

De Boer, T.H., Nefkens, F., van Helvoirt, A. and Delft, A.M. van. 1996. Differences in modulation of noradrenergic and serotonergic transmission by the alpha-2 adrenoceptor antagonists, mirtazapine, mianserin and idazoxan, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 277, pp. 852–860.

Dranovsky, A., Hen, R. 2006. Hippocampal Neurogenesis: Regulation by Stress and Antidepressants, Biol Psychiatry;59:1136–1143.

Drapier, D., Bentué-Ferrer, D., Laviolle, B., Millet, B., Allain, H., Bourin, M., Reymann, J.M. 2007. Effects of acute fluoxetine, paroxetine and desipramine on rats tested on the elevated plus-maze. Behav Brain Res, Jan 25;176(2):202-9.

Dulawa, S.C., Holick, K.A., Gundersen, B., Hen, H. 2004. Effects of Chronic Fluoxetine in Animal Models of Anxiety and Depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1321–1330.

Duman, R.S., Monteggia L.M. 2006. A Neurotrophic Model for Stress-Related Mood Disorders, Biol Psychiatry;59:1116–1127.

Dunlop, B.W., & Davis, P.G. 2008. Combination Treatment With Benzodiazepines and SSRIs for Comorbid Anxiety and Depression: A Review, Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry;10:222–228.

File, S.E. 1990. One-trial tolerance to the anxiolytic effects of chlordiazepoxide in the plus-maze. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 100(2):281-2.

File, S.E., Ouagazzal, A.M., Gonzalez, L.E., Overstreet, D.H. 1999. Chronic fluoxetine in tests of anxiety in rat lines selectively bred for differential 5-HT1A receptor function. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. Apr;62(4):695-701.

Franowicz, J.S., Kessler, L.E., Borja, C.M.D., Kobilka, B.K., Limbird, L.E., Arnsten, A.F.T. 2002. Mutation of the 2A-Adrenoceptor Impairs Working Memory Performance and Annuls Cognitive Enhancement by Guanfacine. The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1, 22(19):8771–8777.

Freud, S. Recordar, repetir e elaborar. 1914, reediction of 1976.

Gabriele, A., Packard, M.G. 2006. Evidence of a role for multiple memory systems in behavioral extinction. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, v.85 p.289– 299.

(49)

49 Griebel, G., Blanchard, D.C., Agnes, R.S., Blanchard, R.J. 1995. Differential modulation of antipredator defensive behavior in Swiss-Webster mice following acute or chronic administration of imipramine and fluoxetine. Psychopharmacology (Berl). Jul;120(1):57-66.

Haddjeri, N., Blier, P., Montigny, C. de. 1996. Effect of the alpha-2 adrenoceptor antagonist mirtazapine on the 5-hydroxytryptamine system in the rat brain, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 277 pp. 861–871)

Hamann, S. 2005. Sex differences in the responses of the human amygdala. The neuroscientist, 11, 288-293.

Hansson, A.C., Rimondini, R., Heilig, M., Mathé, A.A., Sommer, W.H. 2011. Dissociation of antidepressant-like activity of escitalopram and nortriptyline on behaviour and hippocampal BDNF expression in female rats. J Psychopharmacol. Jan 24.

Harmer, C.J., Mackay, C.E., Reid, C.B., Cowen, P.J., Goodwin, G.M. 2006. Antidepressant Drug Treatment Modifies the Neural Processing of Nonconscious Threat Cues. Biol Psychiatry;59:816–820.

Harmer, C.J. 2010. Antidepressant drug action: a neuropsychological perspective. Depression and Anxiety 27: 231–233.

Heim, C., Nemeroff, C.B. 2009. Neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. CNS Spectr. Jan;14(1 Suppl 1):13-24.

Hildebrandt, M.G., Steyerberg, E.W., Stage, K.B., Passchier, J., Kragh-Soerensen P. 2003. Are gender differences important for the clinical effects of antidepressants? Am J Psychiatry ;160:1643–50.

Holmes A, Rodgers RJ. 2003. Prior exposure to the elevated plus-maze sensitizes mice to the acute behavioral effects of fluoxetine and phenelzine. Eur J Pharmacol, 459:221–30.

Homberg JR, Olivier JD, Blom T, Arentsen T, van Brunschot C, Schipper P, Korte-Bouws G, van Luijtelaar G, Reneman L. 2011. Fluoxetine exerts age-dependent effects on behavior and amygdala neuroplasticity in the rat. PLoS One. Jan 31;6(1):e16646.

Jonasson, Z. 2005. Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and memory: a review of behavioral and biological data. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 28, 811- 825.

(50)

50 Kendler, K.S., Thornton, L.M., Prescott, C.A. 2001. Gender differences in the rates of exposure to stressful life events and sensitivity to their depressogenic effects. Am. J. Psychiatry, 158, 587-93.

Kim, J.H., Richardson, R. 2010. New Findings on Extinction of Conditioned Fear Early in Development: Theoretical and Clinical Implications, Biol Psychiatry ;67:297–303.

Krystal, J.H., Neumeister, A. 2009. Noradrenergic and serotonergic mechanisms in the neurobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder and resilience, Brain Research 1293 13-23.

Kornstein, S.G., Schatzberg, A.F., Thase, M.E., Yonkers, K.A., McCullough, J.P., Keitner, G. I. et al. 2000. Gender differences in treatment response to sertraline versus imipramine in chronic depression. Am J Psychiatry;157:1445–52.

Kubzansky, L.D., Koenen, K.C., Jones, C., Eaton, W.W. 2009. A Prospective Study of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms and Coronary Heart Disease in Women. Health Psychol. January; 28(1): 125–130.

Kurt, M., Arik, A.C., Celik, S. 2000. The effects of sertraline and fluoxetine on anxiety in the elevated plus-maze test in mice. J Basic Physiol Pharmacol, 11:173– 80.

Lebrón, K., Milad, M.R., Quirk, G.J. 2004. Delayed Recall of Fear Extinction in Rats With Lesions of Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Learn. Mem. 11: 544-548

MacQueen, G.M., Campbell, S., McEwen, B.S., Macdonald, K., Amano, S., Joffe, R.T., Nahmias, C., Young, L.T. 2003. Course of illness, hippocampal function, and hippocampal volume in major depression. PNAS, February 4, vol. 100, no. 3, 1387–1392.

Mahendran, R., Yap, H.L. 2005. Clinical practice guidelines for depression. Singapore Medical Journal. v.46, n.11, p.610-615.

Marcondes, F.K., Miguel, K.J., Melo, L.L., Spadari-Bratfisch, R.C. 2001. Estrous cycle influences the response of female rats in the elevated plus-maze test. Physiol & Behav 74:435– 440.

Marks, I., Lovell, K., Noshirvani, H., Livanou, M., Thrasher, S. 1998. Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder by Exposure and/or Cognitive Restructuring, Arch Gen Psychiatry, 55:317-325.

McCabe, C., Mishor, Z., Cowen, P.J., Harmer, C.J. 2010. Diminished Neural Processing of Aversive and Rewarding Stimuli During Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Treatment. BIOL PSYCHIATRY;67:439–445.

(51)

51 McFarlane, A.C., Papay, P. 1992. Multiple diagnoses in posttraumatic stress disorder in the victims of a natural disaster. J Nerv Ment Dis;180;498-504.

McNamara, R.K., Able, J.A., Rider, T., Tso, P., Jandacek, R. 2010. Effect of chronic fluoxetine treatment on male and female rat erythrocyte and prefrontal cortex fatty acid composition. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. Oct 1;34(7):1317-21.

Milad, M.R., Igoe, S.A., Lebron-Milad, K., Novales, J.E. 2009. Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience 164:887-895.

Miyamoto, D., Iijima, M., Yamamoto, H., Nomura, H., Matsuki, N. 2010. Behavioural effects of antidepressants are dependent and independent on the integrity of the dentate gyrus. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. Nov 3:1-10.

Mirza NR, Nielsen EØ, Troelsen KB. 2007. Serotonin transporter density and anxiolytic-like effects of antidepressants in mice. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. May 9;31(4):858-66.

Monti, B., Berteotti, C., Contestabile, A. 2006. Subchronic rolipram delivery activates hippocampal CREB and arc, enhances retention and slows down extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 278 e 286.

Morgan, M.A., LeDoux, J.E. 1995. Differential Contribution of Dorsal and Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex to the Acquisition and Extinction of Conditioned Fear in Rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 109, No. 4, 681-688

Mueller, E.M., Hofmann, S.G., Cherry, J.A. 2010. The type IV phosphodiesterase inhibitor rolipram disturbs expression and extinction of conditioned fear in mice. Neuropharmacology 59; 1 e 8.

Nandam, L.S., Jhaveri, D., Bartlett, P. 2007. 5-HT7, neurogenesis and antidepressants: a promising therapeutic axis for treating depression, Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and Physiology 34, 546–551.

Nestler, E.J., Barrot, M., DiLeone, R.J., Eisch, A.J., Gold, S.J., Monteggia, L.M. 2002. Neurobiology of depression. Neuron; 34:13–25.

Olivier, J.D., Van Der Hart, M.G., Van Swelm, R.P., Dederen, P.J., Homberg, J.R., Cremers, T., Deen, P.M., Cuppen, E., Cools, A.R., Ellenbroek, B.A. 2008. A study in male and female 5-HT transporter knockout rats: an animal model for anxiety and depression disorders. Neuroscience. Mar 27;152(3):573-84.

(52)

52 Pittenger, C. & Duman, R.S. Stress, 2008. Depression, and Neuroplasticity: A Convergence of Mechanisms, Neuropsychopharmacology REVIEWS 33, 88–109.

Porsolt, R.D., Le Pichon, M., Jalfre, M. 1977. Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatment. Nature, v. 266, p.730-732.

Porsolt, R.D., Anton, G., Blavet, N., Jalfre, M. 1978. Behavioural despair in rats: a new model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Eur J Pharmacol ;47:379– 91.

Quirk, G.J., Garcia, R., González-Lima, F. 2006. Prefrontal Mechanisms in Extinction of Conditioned Fear. BIOL PSYCHIATRY;60:337–343

Rauch, S.L., Shin, L.M., Phelps, E.A. 2006. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research – past, present, and future. Biol. Psychiatry, 60, 376-382.

Rauggi, R., Cassanelli, A., Raone, A., Tagliamonte, A., Gambarana, C. 2005. Study of mirtazapine antidepressant effects in rats. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, v.8, p. 369–379.

Rawlings, N.B., Norbury, R., Cowen, P.J., Harmer C.J. 2010. A single dose of mirtazapine modulates neural responses to emotional faces in healthy people. Psychopharmacology 01 September.

Reneric, J.P., Bouvard, M., Stinus, L. 2002. In the rat forced swimming test, chronic but not subacute administration of dual 5-HT/NA antidepressant treatments may produce greater effects than selective drugs. Behav Brain Res 136: 521–532.

Ribeiro, A.M, Barbosa, F.F, Godinho, M.R, Fernandes, V.S, Munguba, H., Alves, L.M, Melo, T.G, Barbosa, M.T, Silva, R.H. 2010. Sex differences in aversive memory in rats: possible role of extinction and reactive emotional factors. Brain and Cognition Volume 74, Issue 2, November, Pages 145-151.

Rilea, S.L., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., Boles, D. 2004. Sex differences in spatial ability: a lateralization of function approach. Brain and Cognition, 56, 332-343.

Robert G, Drapier D, Bentué-Ferrer D, Renault A, Reymann JM. 2011. Acute and chronic anxiogenic-like response to fluoxetine in rats in the elevated plus-maze: Modulation by stressful handling. Behav Brain Res. Jul 7;220(2):344-8.

Rodgers, R.J., Cao, B.-J., Dalvi, A., Holmes, A. 1997. Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective Braz J Med Biol Res 30(3).

Rodgers, R.J., Dalvi, A. 1997. Anxiety, Defence and the Elevated Plus-maze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, Vol. 21, No. 6, pp. 801–810.

(53)

53 Rogóż, Z. 2010. Effects of co-treatment with mirtazapine and low doses of risperidone on immobility time in the forced swimming test in mice. Pharmacol Rep. Nov-Dec;62(6):1191-6.

Sahay, A. & Hen R. 2007. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression, Nature Neuroscience v.10; 9; september.

Santarelli, L., Saxe, M., Gross, C., Battaglia, F., Dulawa, S., Weisstaub, N., Lee, J., Duman, R., Arancio, O., Belzung, C., Hen, R. 2003. Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effectc of antidepressants. Science vol 301, 8 August.

Silva, M.T., Alves, C.R., Santarem, E.M. 1999. Anxiogenic-like effect of acute and chronic fluoxetine on rats tested on the elevated plus-maze. Braz J Med Biol Res, 32:333–9.

Silva, R.H., Bellot, R.G., Vital, M.A. & Frussa-Filho, R. 1997. Effects of long-term ganglioside GM1 administration on a new discriminative avoidance test in normal adult mice. Psychopharmacology, 129, 322-328.

Silva, R.H. & Frussa-Filho, R. 2000. The plus-maze discriminative acoidance task: a new model to study memory-anxiety interactions. Effects of chlordiazepoxide and caffeine. J Neurosci. Methods, 102, 117-125.

Silva, R.H., Abílio, V.C., Torres-Leite, D., Bergamo, M., Chinen, C.C., Claro, F.T., Carvalho, R.C. & Frussa-Filho, R. 2002a. Concomitant development of oral dyskinesia and memory deficits in reserpine-treated male and female mice. Behav. Brain. Res., 132, 171-177.

Silva, R.H., Kameda, S.R., Carvalho, R.C., Rigo, G.S., Costa, K.L., Taricano, I.D. & Frussa-Filho, R. 2002b. Effects of amphetamine on the plus-maze discriminative avoidance task in mice. Psychopahrmacology, 160, 9-18.

Silva, R.H., Chehin, A.B., Kameda, S.R., Takatsu-Coleman, A.L., Abilio, V.C., Tufik, S. & Frussa-Filho, R. 2004. Effects of pre- or post-training paradoxical sleep deprivation on two animal models of learning and memory in mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem., 82, 90-98.

Solomon, M.B., Herman, J.P. 2009. Sex differences in psychopathology: Of gonads, adrenals and mental illness, Physiology & Behavior 97 250–258.

Schüle, C., Baghai, T., Goy, J., Bidlingmaier, M., Strasburger, C., Laakmann, G. 2002. The influence of mirtazapine on anterior pituitary hormone secretion in healthy male subjects. Psychopharmacology 163:95–101

(54)

54 Schüle, C., Baghai, T.C., Eser, D., Zwanzger, P., Jordan, M., Buechs, R., Rupprecht, R. 2006. Time course of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis activity during treatment with reboxetine and mirtazapine in depressed patients. Psychopharmacology (Berl). Jul;186(4):601-11.

Schulz, D., Buddenberg, T., Huston, J.P. 2007. Extinction-induced ―despair‖ in the water maze, exploratory behavior and fear: Effects of chronic antidepressant treatment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 87; 624–634.

Toufexis, D.J., Myers, K.M., Davis, M. 2006. The effect of gonadal hormones and gender on anxiety and emotional learning, Hormones and Behavior 50 539–549.

Varea, E., Castillo-Gómez, E., Gómez-Climent, M.A., Blasco-Ibáñez, J.M., Crespo, C., Martínez-Guijarro, F.J., Nàcher, J. 2007. Chronic antidepressant treatment induces contrasting patterns of synaptophysin and PSA-NCAM expression in different regions of the adult rat telencephalon. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. Jul;17(8):546-57.

Wald, C., & Wu C. 2010. Biomedical research. Of mice and women: the bias in animal models. Science, Mar 26;327 (5973):1571-2.

Woolley, D.G., Vermaercke, B. Beeck, H. O. de, Wagemans, J., Gantois, I., D‘Hooge, R. Swinnen, S. P., Wenderoth, N. 2010. Sex differences in human virtual water maze performance: Novel measures reveal the relative contribution of directional responding and spatial knowledge. Behavioural Brain Research 208 408– 414.

Yamamoto, S., Morinobu, S., Fuchikami, M., Kurata, A., Kozuru, T., Yamawaki, S. 2008. Effects of Single Prolonged Stress and D-Cycloserine on Contextual Fear Extinction and Hippocampal NMDA Receptor Expression in a Rat Model of PTSD. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 2108–2116

Yamamoto, S., Morinobu, S., Takei, S., Fuchikami, M., Matsuki, A., Yamawaki, S., Liberzon, I. 2009. Single prolonged stress: toward an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety.;26 (12):1110-7.

(55)

55 AUTHOR INFORMATION PACK

6 Jan 2011 www.elsevier.com/locate/pnp 1

PROGRESS IN NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY & BIOLOGICAL PSYCHIATRY

An International Research, Review, and News Journal

AUTHOR INFORMATION PACK

TABLE OF CONTENTS

• Description

• Audience

• Impact Factor

• Abstracting and Indexing

• Editorial Board

• Guide for Authors

ISSN: 0278-5846

DESCRIPTION .

(56)

56 experimental and clinical aspects of neuro-psychopharmacology and biological psychiatry. Another important aim of the journal is to supply pertinent information, provided by national and international bodies, that contributes to progress in the scientific and professional fields. Finally, the journal intends to foster and encourage communications between members of the communities of neuro-psychopharmacology and biological psychiatry.

Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry does not publish work on the actions of biological extracts unless the pharmacological active molecular substrate and/or specific receptor binding properties of the extract compounds are elucidated.

US National Institutes of Health (NIH) voluntary posting ("Public Access") policy

Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry and Elsevier facilitate the author's response to the NIH Public Access Policy. For more details please see the Guide for authors

(57)

57 via e-mail for publication by Elsevier in various Elsevier journals and other publications. Ultimately, these fraudulent e-mails involve a request for the victims to send "handling fees" to cover the processing of the article that has been submitted.

Please be assured that Elsevier, Inc. is in no way associated with this fraudulent e-mail campaign. In addition, please be advised that Elsevier does not solicit intellectual property from authors in this fashion, and does not utilize Gmail, Hotmail, or any other free thirdparty e-mail providers in communications with authors and editors.

AUTHOR INFORMATION PACK 6 Jan 2011 www.elsevier.com/locate/pnp 2

If you receive any e-mail messages that appear to be a part of this fraudulent solicitation, DO NOT respond to the message and do not open any attachments contained in the message. Rather, please forward the message to Elsevier's Fraud Department at emailabuse@elsevier.com. We will use the information included in the message to aid in our investigation.

AUDIENCE .

Neuroscientists, pharmacologists, psychiatrists, psychologists in both basic and clinical research.

IMPACT FACTOR .

(58)

58 ABSTRACTING AND INDEXING

.

BIOSIS

Cambridge Scientific Abstracts Chemical Abstracts

Current Contents/BIOMED Database

Current Contents/Index to Scientific Reviews Current Contents/Life Sciences

EMBASE

Elsevier BIOBASE MEDLINE®

Research Alert SCISEARCH

Science Citation Index Scopus

EDITORIAL BOARD .

Editor-in-Chief:

Guy Drolet, Neurosciences, Centre de Recherche du CHUL (CHUQ), RC-9800 2705 Laurier, Quebec, G1V 4G2,

Canada

Assistant Editor:

Sylvie Laforest,

(59)

59 Paul Bédard,

Founding Editor:

C. Radouco-Thomas*,

Editorial Board:

O. Akyol, Ankara, Turkey H. Belmaker, Beersheva, Israel P. Blier, Ottawa, Canada

M-H. Bloch, New Haven, CT, USA C. Davidson, London, UK

S.I. Deutsch, Washington, DC, USA K. Domschke, Münster, Germany F. Graeff, Ribeirao Preto SP, Brazil L. He, Shanghai, China

F. Holsboer, München, Germany A.V. Kalueff, Tampere, Finland K. Kasai, Tokyo, Japan

T. Kato, Wako, Japan

Y.-K. Kim, Ansan City, South Korea

AUTHOR INFORMATION PACK 6 Jan 2011 www.elsevier.com/locate/pnp 3 S.K. Kulkarni, Chandigarh, India

(60)

60 C. Rouillard, Quebec, QC, Canada

J. Samochowiec, Szczecin, Poland A. Serretti, Bologna, Italy

R. Silva, Natal, Brazil

H. Singh Duggal, Tecumseh, MI, USA D. Stein, Cape Town, South Africa R. Szeszko, Glen Oaks, NY, USA S. Tsai, Taipei, Taiwan, ROC P. Vezina, Chicago, IL, USA M. Xu, Boston, MA, USA

X-Y. Zhang, Houston, TX, USA

AUTHOR INFORMATION PACK 6 Jan 2011 www.elsevier.com/locate/pnp 4 GUIDE FOR AUTHORS

.

INTRODUCTION

(61)

61 Studies on natural products The journal does not publish work on the actions of biological extracts unless the pharmacological active molecular substrate and/or specific receptor binding properties of the extract compounds are elucidated.

Types of paper

1. Original research articles

2. Review articles: Mini-reviews or comprehensive reviews of cutting edge work, or syntheses of cutting edge work that has been done in the past two years

As of 1st December 2010, Letters to the Editor are no longer accepted for publication in the journal.

Page charges

This journal has no page charges. BEFORE YOU BEGIN

Ethics in Publishing

For information on Ethics in Publishing and Ethical guidelines for journal publication

see http://www.elsevier.com/publishingethics and

http://www.elsevier.com/ethicalguidelines.

Policy and ethics

The work described in your article must have been carried out in accordance with

The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) for

experiments involving humans

(62)

62

86/609/EEC for animal experiments

http://ec.europa.eu/environment/chemicals/lab_animals/legislation_en.htm; Uniform

Requirements for manuscripts submitted to Biomedical journals http://www.icmje.org. This must be stated at an appropriate point in the article.

Conflict of interest

All authors are requested to disclose any actual or potential conflict of interest including any financial, personal or other relationships with other people or organizations within three years of beginning the submitted work that could inappropriately influence, or be perceived to influence, their work. See also

http://www.elsevier.com/conflictsofinterest.

Submission declaration

Submission of an article implies that the work described has not been published previously (except in the form of an abstract or as part of a published lecture or academic thesis), that it is not under consideration for publication elsewhere, that its publication is approved by all authors and tacitly or explicitly by the responsible authorities where the work was carried out, and that, if accepted, it will not be published elsewhere including electronically in the same form, in English or in any other language, without the written consent of the copyright-holder.

Contributors

(63)

63 so roles for all authors should be described. The statement that all authors have approved the final article should be true and included in the disclosure.

Changes to authorship

This policy concerns the addition, deletion, or rearrangement of author names in the authorship of accepted manuscripts:

AUTHOR INFORMATION PACK 6 Jan 2011 www.elsevier.com/locate/pnp 5

Before the accepted manuscript is published in an online issue: Requests to add or

remove an author, or to rearrange the author names, must be sent to the Journal Manager from the corresponding author of the accepted manuscript and must include: (a) the reason the name should be added or removed, or the author names rearranged and (b) written confirmation (e-mail, fax, letter) from all authors that they agree with the addition, removal or rearrangement. In the case of addition or removal of authors, this includes confirmation from the author being added or removed. Requests that are not sent by the corresponding author will be forwarded by the Journal Manager to the corresponding author, who must follow the procedure as described above. Note that: (1) Journal Managers will inform the Journal Editors of any such requests and (2) publication of the accepted manuscript in an online issue is suspended until authorship has been agreed.

After the accepted manuscript is published in an online issue: Any requests to add,

delete, or rearrange author names in an article published in an online issue will follow the same policies as noted above and result in a corrigendum.

Referências

Documentos relacionados

Fluoxetine induced an anxiogenic-like effect in rats tested in the elevated plus- maze: the drug decreased the proportion of open arm time and entries, regardless of whether it

Several results from our laboratory have suggested the possibility that the impact of LTH on the first trial or on the first 2-trial block of the testing session (initial testing

Thus, the present experiment was de- signed to study the correlation of behavior of the same mice tested in the elevated plus- maze and in the forced swimming test using the

On the basis of the present observation that the 1.5 mg/kg dose of diaze- pam tended to increase the number of open arm entries by Lewis rats, it may be con- cluded that Lewis rats

Twenty animals of each group were used in the object recognition task (10 in short-term memory and 10 in long-term memory), 12 in the plus-maze test and 12 in the forced swimming

The present study showed a high frequency of VD in patients with pacemakers submitted to long-term right ventricular apical pacing who were clinically stable, in addition to

In the present study, we investigated the effects of chronic treatment with fl uoxetine in rats treated with oxaliplatin and evaluated the possible quantitative alterations in

The present study aimed to assess renal repair in rats who had renal infarction induced by the obstruction of blood flow in the renal artery and were treated