• Nenhum resultado encontrado

CoDAS vol.25 número3

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2018

Share "CoDAS vol.25 número3"

Copied!
6
0
0

Texto

(1)

Artigo Original

Bárbara Backes1 Regina Basso Zanon1 Cleonice Alves Bosa1

Keywords

Autistic disorder Language Social behavior Communication

Descritores

Transtorno autístico Linguagem Comportamento social Comunicação

Correspondence address:

Bárbara Backes

R. Ramiro Barcelos, 2.600, Porto Alegre (RS), Brasil, CEP: 90035-003. E-mail: barbara.edas@gmail.com

Received: 08/19/2012

Accepted: 02/18/2013

Study carried out at the Nucleus of Study and Research in Developmental Disorders (NIEPED), Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul – UFRGS – Porto Alegre (RS), Brazil, with fellowship from Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) and from the Fullbright Program.

(1) Post-graduation Program in Psychology, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul – UFRGS – Porto Alegre (RS), Brazil.

Conlict of interest: nothing to declare.

social communicative development of children

with autism spectrum disorder

A relação entre regressão da linguagem e desenvolvimento

sociocomunicativo de crianças com transtorno

do espectro do autismo

ABSTRACT

Purpose: To investigate the relationship between language regression and the subsequent social-communicative development of preschool children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Methods: Thirty children with ASD participated in the study and were divided into two groups: with (n= 6) and without (n= 24) language regression. Language regression was assessed by the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised and the social-communicative development was measured by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Results: Of the 30 children who met the criteria for participation in this study, six (20%) had regression of oral language skills, with a mean age of onset of 25 months. There were no statistical signiicant differences in the social-communicative development between the groups with and without language regression. Conclusion: The indings of this research do not seem to conirm the relationship between the occurrence of language regression and the subsequent impairment on social-communicative development of children with ASD.

RESUMO

Objetivo: Investigar a relação entre a ocorrência de regressão da linguagem e o desenvolvimento sociocomunicativo posterior de crianças pré-escolares com Transtorno do Espectro do Autismo (TEA).

Métodos: Participaram do estudo 30 crianças com TEA, divididas em dois grupos: com (n=6) e sem regressão da linguagem (n=24). A regressão da linguagem foi avaliada com base na Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised

(2)

INTRODUCTION

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition that presents in the irst years of a child’s life(1,2).

The term ASD has been used in publications to cover autistic disorders, Asperger’s syndrome, childhood disintegrative disorders and the pervasive developmental disorder not oth-erwise speciied. The etiology of ASD is still unknown, but research has pointed to genetic and neurobiological correlates(3).

Furthermore, epidemiological data show a prevalence of one in 150 births and this has increased in recent decades. This can be explained by the expansion of the diagnostic criteria, the increase of health services related to the disorder and the change in the age of diagnosis, among other factors(4).

ASD is characterized by the presence of social-commu-nicative impairments and restricted and repetitive behavior patterns (1). It is important to note that the DSM-IV-TR(5) still

uses three diagnostic dimensions since it does not integrates the social interaction and communication skills. These, along with repetitive and stereotyped behaviors, constitute the “di-agnostic triad”. The degree of impairment in these areas varies signiicantly(6). According to the diagnostic criteria found in the

DSM-V (http://www.dsm5.org), changes in the commu-nicative dimension for example, relect dificulties in social-emotional reciprocity in verbal and nonverbal communication behaviors and in establishing and maintaining relationships. The presence of restricted and repetitive behaviors can manifest through stereotypes and repetitions of motor movements, use of objects and speech, as well as restricted interests, excessive and rigid adherence to routines and hypo- or hypersensitivity to sensory inputs(1).

Speciically on language, delays in the acquisition and development of this ability are common in individuals with ASD and the linguistic impairments in these individuals may be present in morphology, phonology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics(7). In the Brazilian context, some researches that

investigated the formal aspects of oral language of individuals with ASD are highlighted. For example, one study examined the use of verbal communication skills to promote increased mean length of utterance in the context of rehabilitation. Participants were three male individuals with ASD who were 12 years old. The intervention was effective in all three cases, stressing the importance of developing strategies to improve the communicative competence of individuals with the disorder(8).

Another study has investigated the evolution of the functional and grammatical aspects of language in ten boys with ASD with ages between 2.7 and 11.2 years. These aspects were measured and compared at three time points: at initial assessment, after six and 12 months of rehabilitation. Although there were no statistically signiicant differences between variables over the period evaluated, some of these variables were correlated (e.g., verbs, interpersonal and gestural acts), which denotes the relationship between vocabulary, grammar and pragmatics(9).

A more recent research has examined the attribution of mental states, vocabulary and sentence length in the speech of ten children with ASD between ive and 11 years of age. This analysis was performed at three different times in order

to evaluate possible changes resulting from rehabilitation. The results showed that the participants used verbs that denote mental states in speech, but increase in usage of verbs was not observed over the study period. On the other hand, there was an expansion of vocabulary and sentence length of the participants, pointing to the importance of speech and language therapy(10).

A different study investigated the pragmatic abilities of 14 children with ASD during interaction with their teachers in classrooms of mainstream education. Participants were aged between three and eight years. The results showed that gesture was the most used mean in the communicative act. Moreover, as to the communicative functions, a higher occurrence of less interactive ones, such as naming, was observed. The authors also highlighted the existence of a positive correlation between the verbal mode and more interactive communicative func-tions, as well as between the gestural mode and less interactive functions(11).

Thus, language is a very relevant ield regarding ASD. However, there are still aspects relating to language develop-ment in children with the disorder that are poorly understood, as is the case of regression. Therefore, the interest of researchers on regression in language abilities in some cases of ASD has recently increased.

Although there is no consensus on the operational deini-tion of regression of language, it tends to be deined as loss of communicative use of three to ive words, except for “papa” and “mama”. In addition, both the time before the regression as the duration of loss should be less than three months(12-14).

The etiology of regression is still unknown, but the biological correlates related to epilepsy have been identiied(15) as well as

the existence of genetic predispositions(16).

There is evidence that between 15 and 33% of children with ASD have regression of some skill, as in social, play or language(12,17) and the age of onset of regression varies between

12 and 36 months(18,19). Moreover, there are indications that

the loss of skills is more frequent in individuals with autism speciically as compared to other spectrum disorders(12,14,20,21).

Some studies also show that the oral language skill is the most commonly affected domain by the phenomenon of regres-sion(12,17,21), occurring in approximate 20% of individuals with

ASD. Still, the regression of oral language is often associated with loss of social skills(13,17,21).

There are recurring empirical efforts that aim to determine whether the regression of oral language presents itself as a risk factor for later development of children with ASD. In this state-ment, while some studies support this hypothesis(12,22,23), others

have found no relationship between the regression of language and greater developmental impairment(20,21,24).

(3)

social skills was analyzed. The loss of social-communicative skill proved to be characteristic of ASD, and the group with regression showed greater impairment in all areas investigated as compared to those without regression. Among these impair-ments, greater severity of symptoms in all areas of the triad and larger deicits in social skills, especially in relation to the ap-propriateness of behavior to the social context, were reported(22).

A different study conducted in the UK investigated the cor-relation between regression of language, autistic symptoms and course of development of 255 children with and without ASD, with an average age of 10.3 years. The average age at which regression occurred was 25 months — moment at which the chil-dren produced their irst simple phrases. The loss of oral language skills proved to be characteristic of the group with the disorder. The results also revealed that the group of children with ASD and historical regression showed more severe autistic symptoms compared to those with the disorder and without regression(12).

Another study conducted in the United States speciically investigated the regression of language emphasizing the as-sociated behaviors and the consequences of this phenomenon. Participants were 196 children with loss of language skills, whose average age was 50.6 months at the time of the study. It was observed that regression of language was usually ac-companied by loss of social skills and play. Regarding the language level before the regression, most of the children were in the period of acquisition of phrases, with 78% of the children acquiring the irst words before 18 months. It was also observed that 73% of participants had cognitive skills below those ex-pected for their age. The researchers concluded that regression of language skills in childhood seems to be a phenomenon that adversely affects the child’s overall development, pointing to the investigative importance of the topic(23).

In contrast to the previously presented studies, a study conducted in the United States found no relationship between the presence of regression of language and severity of autistic symptoms. The clinical characteristics associated with re-gression of language skills in 114 children with ASD with a mean age of 41.4 months were investigated. As examined in other studies, the loss of language skills occurred in 29.8% of participants, at an average age of 19.5 months. However, no signiicant differences were observed between participants with and without regression with regard to the severity of symptoms in the three diagnostic areas (social interaction, communication and repetitive and stereotyped behaviors)(20).

In the same line of investigation, another study conducted in the United States examined the possible relationship between regression of social-communication skill and behavioral char-acteristics (e.g., symptoms, adaptive behavior) at three and four years. The study included 72 children with ASD, with a mean age of 25.2 months. The group with regression showed greater severity of symptoms only in the dimension of social interac-tion assessed by Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). There were no signiicant differences between the groups regard-ing the severity of symptoms in the areas of communication and

repetitive and stereotyped behaviors. In addition, the groups did not differ on measures of verbal and nonverbal intelligence quotient and aberrant and adaptive behaviors(24).

Another study also developed in the United States investi-gated the existence of differences in the overall development of individuals with ASD with and without regression of linguistic and non-linguistic skills (e.g., nonverbal social interaction). The study included 573 children with a mean age of 7.9 years, and of these, 237 were diagnosed with autism, 199 with another spec-trum disorder and 137 with developmental delay. There were no signiicant differences in the presence of autistic symptoms among participants with and without a history of regression(21).

In summary, it is observed that the relationship between regression and subsequent development of individuals with ASD is still controversial. Thus, the present study aimed to investigate the relationship between regression of oral language and later social-communicative development of preschool children with ASD. The purpose was to compare the social and communicative development of children with the disorder who showed or not regression of oral language.

METHODS

Study Design and Participants

A retrospective study with contrasting groups was carried out(25). One group was composed of children diagnosed with

autistic disorder with a history of regression of oral language skills (Group 1) and the other of children with the same diag-nosis but without a history of regression (Group 2). The groups were matched by gender, race and language level. Thus, the sampled children were preschool, caucasians, diagnosed with autistic disorder and had complete data concerning the measures investigated. The exclusion criteria consisted on the presence of associated physical and sensory disabilities as well as other disorders in the spectrum.

This study is part of a project (CAPES / Fulbright, 2009-101)

that investigates predictors of social development in children with ASD, using a database composed of 150 children in pre-school age, patients from the Cincinnatti Children’s Medical Center (CCHMC). It consists on a clipping from the general database of CCHMC which was especially authorized for the realization of this project.

This study was inanced by Capes/Fulbright and devel-oped at the Graduate Program in Psychlogy, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul in partnership with The Kelly O’Leary Center for Autism Spectrum DisordersDivision of Developmental DisabilitiesCincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Ohio, USA.

Instruments

The ADI-R(26) was used to evaluate the regression of

oral language. It consists on a standardized semi-structured

(4)

interview administered to parents or caregivers of children with suspected ASD. It has 93 items that investigate social and communicative impairments and the presence of repeti-tive and stereotyped behaviors, both in relation to the irst ive years of a child’s life, as the current behavior, referring to the 12 months preceding the interview. In this study, items 11 to 28 of the ADI-R were used to speciically investigate the oc-currence of regression.

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)

(27) is a standardized observational and semi-structured

instrument that investigates the social interaction skills, communication, play and repetitive and stereotyped be-haviors. In this study, only the dimensions that assess the development of social-communicative skills were used — i.e. those related to social interaction and communication. The ADOS was selected for being an independent measure of the ADI-R and because the observation is made from standardized situations. The behaviors are encoded in scores from 0 to 2 or 0 to 3 depending on the item, being 0 a less committed behavior and 2 or 3 more unusual behav-iors. The ADOS consists of four modules for the different levels of expressive language. These modules relate to, respectively, (1) pre-verbal behaviors and isolated words and simple sentences, (2) non-luent production of sentences, and (3 and 4) luent speech. In this study, the inal scores of the algorithm in the dimensions of social interaction and communication in Modules 1, 2 and 3 were used since the fourth module concerns adolescents and adults.

The ADI-R and ADOS instruments are considered the gold standard for having been validated in several countries while maintaining its sensitivity and diagnostic accuracy. Thus, the interviewer must undergo prior training that enables the man-agement and coding of the results.

Procedures and Ethical Considerations

The author of the main project, of which this study is part, obtained formal authorization from CCHMC for the use of the database preserving the anonymity of the participants, who were identiied by codes.

RESULTS

Of the 30 children who met the criteria for participation in this study, six (20%) had regression of oral language skills. The average age of onset of loss was 25 months (SD=6.19), with a mean duration of 16.3 months (SD=4.24). Regarding the level of language before the regression, ive children (83.3%) were using at least ive words spontaneously and frequently, and one (16.7%) was using words spontaneously only on an occasional basis. It was reported that four children (66.7%) who had language regression also lost skills of social interaction, as for example, interest and social involvement. The other two chil-dren (33.3%) showed no loss in other areas beyond language. There was no co-occurrence between the loss of language skills and organic diseases, such as epilepsy, as investigated by item 18 of the ADI-R.

Thus, six children were assigned to regression group (Group 1) and 24 to the no regression group (Group 2). At the time of assessment, the average age of Group 1 was 4.83 (SD=0.33) and of Group 2 3.91 (SD=0.15). The Mann-Whitney U test showed that children with regression of oral language were older than those without such a history, and this difference was statistically signiicant (U=134, p=0.004).

Given that ADOS is divided into modules according to the language level of the child, the distribution of the different modules was carried out in the two groups (Table 1). The results showed that participants in both groups tended to concentrate in Modules 1 and 2, that is, they were children whose language level ranged from pre-verbal behaviors to sentence production. This distribution was

Group Module 1* n (%)

Module 2** n (%)

Total n (%)

1 2 (33.3) 4 (66.7) 6 (100)

2 12 (50) 12 (50) 24 (100)

*Pre-verbal behaviors and isolated words; **sentence production

Table 1. Distribution of groups with (G1) and without (G2) regression in

ADOS modules” por “in the ADOS modules

Table 2. Mean final scores in the investigated domains according to group

Domains Module 1

M (SD)

Module 2 M (SD) COM

Group 1 (with regression) Group 2 (without regression)

6.00 (1.00) 5.91 (0.60)

6.00 (1.22) 6.00 (0.39) INT

Group 1 (with regression) Group 2 (without regression)

8.00 (3.00) 8.91 (0.79)

8.50 (0.50) 9.25 (0.58) COM/INT

Group 1 (with regression) Group 2 (without regression)

14.00 (2.00) 14.83 (1.20)

14.50 (1.55) 15.25 (0.83) Legend: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; COM = communication do-main; INT = social interaction dodo-main; COM/INT = communication and social interaction domains

made because the items of each module are formulated according to the language level of the child with different cutoff points. It is therefore pertinent to inform that the cutoffs for Autistic Disorder in Modules 1 and 2 of the ADOS in the dimensions of communica-tion, social interaction and the combined total of both areas are, respectively, 4 and 5; 7 and6; 12 and 12.

Based on this distribution, the average of the inal scores were then obtained in the dimensions investigated, separated by module and group, as shown in Table 2. From the data analysis relating to the inal scores, the averages were above the cutoff point for autistic disorder in all dimensions investigated. Thus, this inding shows that both groups (with and without regres-sion) have signiicant social and communicative impairment.

(5)

level≤0.05. The total of dimensions of communication (COM) and social interaction (INT) were compared separately, as well as the combined total of both dimensions (COM/INT).

There was no statistically signiicant difference in any of the analyses, indicating that the level of social and communicative commitment was similar in patients with and without regression of oral language skills. The results of these comparisons are presented separately, according to the ADOS module.

Thus, in Module 1, the comparison between the groups with and without regression in the communication dimension resulted in U=11 (p>0.05), and the dimension of social inter-action U=10 (p>0, 05). When both dimensions were analyzed together, the result was U=9 (p>0.05).

With respect to module 2, the comparison between groups with and without regression in the dimensions of social in-teraction and communication resulted, respectively, in U=23 (p>0.05) and U=17.5 (p>0.05). In turn, after the combined comparative analysis of both dimensions, the result found was U=19.5 (p>0.05). Table 3 shows the values of Z and p in each of the analyses.

development of individuals with ASD(12,22). This controversy

highlights the fact that nature of the regression and its conse-quences for the overall development of children still remain poorly understood, in addition to point to methodological limitations of the different studies - such as the small number of participants and the use of non-standardized tools for the investigation of regression.

CONCLUSION

The indings of this study seem to not conirm the relation-ship between the occurrence of regression of oral language and subsequent impairment in the social-communicative development of children with ASD. Although this result is in line with previous research, some methodological points should be addressed. First, the small number of participants with regression of language is highlighted, which precluded the use of parametric tests, which are more statistically robust, inluencing therefore the effect found. Moreover, it was not possible to pair the groups by chronological age. Although all the children were at the same age range, the statistical analysis showed that the age difference between the groups with and without language regression was sig-niicant. Moreover, the limitation of the use of the database, which restricts access to additional information such as the cognitive ability of the participants, the type of words lost and recovered, the frequency and duration of speech therapy are emphasized. These and other information could assist in understanding the phenomenon.

As a suggestion for future studies, the development of re-search with more expressive samples as well as with Brazilian participants is emphasized. Furthermore, longitudinal studies that enable researchers to assess the impact of regression on the global development of the child more accurately should be considered. At the qualitative research line, case studies are also desirable for understanding the context of the language loss in families.

Finally, the importance of understanding these indings based on theories of language development — as, for ex-ample, from the perspective of social pragmatics approach — is highlighted(30). This understanding is critical for speech

and language therapy, considering that most studies are epidemiological.

ACKOWLDGEMENTS

We thank Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) and Fulbright for their support to carry out this research. We are also grateful to Patricia Manning-Courtney, MD, Associate Professor of Clinical Pediatrics, University of Cincinnati.

*BB was responsible for the study project and delimitation, data tabulation, analysis and interpretation and for the manuscript writing; RBZ collaborated with data analysis and interpretation, besides the critical review of the manuscript; CAB carried out the project elaboration and was responsible for the general orientation of the manuscript elaboration and execution..

Table 3. Between-group comparison of mean socores in the

investi-gated dimensions

Dimensions Module 1

z (p-value)*

Module 2 z (p-value)*

COM -0.18 (0.66) -0.12 (0.95)

INT -0.38 (0.79) -0.81 (0.45)

COM/INT -0.55 (0.92) -0.55 (0.60)

* Mann-Whitney U test

Legend: M = mean; SD = standard deviation; COM = communication domain; INT = social interaction domain; COM/INT = communication and social interac-tion domains

DISCUSSION

The results showed that 20% of children showed regression of language skills in the irst three years of life. This inding is close to the proportion found in previous studies(12,14,19,21).

However, it is observed that there is still no consensus on the prevalence of regression in ASD, as several factors seem to inluence its establishment such as the conceptual deinition, the sample size and the subgroup studied(28).

The co-occurrence of oral language regression and loss of social skills was observed in the majority of children in this study, which has also been reported in previous indings(12,28).

Furthermore, co-occurrence of regression of language and epilepsy was not identiied, in line with other studies(12,20,28),

although this association has been previously shown(29).

It should be noted that the results of this study showed no statistically signiicant difference between groups with regard to the investigated behaviors in the dimensions of communication and social interaction. This means that, in the current study, the regression of oral language skills did not seem to present as a risk factor for later social-communicative development in individuals with ASD. While corroborating previous studies(21,24), this inding also agrees with others

(6)

REFERENCES

1. APA: American Psychiatric Association [Internet]. DSM-5 Development [Technical Report]. [cited 2012 Jan 3] Available from: http://www.dsm5. org/Pages/Default.aspx

2. Wing L. The continuum of autistic characteristics. In: Schopler E, Mesibov GB, orgs. Diagnosis and assessment in autism. New York: Plenum Press, 1996. p. 91-110.

3. Rutter ML. Progress in understanding autism: 2007-2010. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41(4):395-404.

4. Fombonne E. Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatr Res.2009;65(6):591-8.

5. APA: American Psychiatric Association. DSM-IV-TR: Manual diagnóstico e estatístico de transtornos mentais. 4a ed. Porto Alegre: Artes

Médicas; 2002.

6. Volden J, Coolican J, Garon N, White J, Bryson S. Pragmatic language in autism spectrum disorder: relationships to measures of ability and disability. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(2):388-93.

7. Eigsti IM, Marchena AB, Schuh JM, Kelley E. Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorders: a developmental review. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2011;5(2):681-91.

8. Lopes-Herrera SA, Almeida MA. O uso de habilidades comunicativas verbais para aumento da extensão de enunciados no autismo de alto funcionamento e na Síndrome de Asperger. Pró-Fono R. Atual. Cient. 2008;20(1):37-42.

9. Miilher LP, Fernandes FDM. habilidades pragmáticas, vocabulares e gramaticais em crianças com transtornos do espectro autístico. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2009;21(4):309-14.

10. Rodrigues LCC, Tamanaha AC, Perissinoto J. Atribuição de estados mentais no discurso de crianças do espectro autístico. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;16(1):25-9.

11. Brito MC, Carrara K. Alunos com distúrbios do espectro autístico em interação com professores na educação inclusiva: descrição de habilidades pragmáticas. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2010;15(3):421-9. 12. Baird G, Charman T, Pickles A, Chandler S, Loucas T, Meldrum D, et

al. Regression, developmental trajectory and associated problems in disorders in the autism spectrum: the SNAP study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(10):1827-36.

13. Lord C, Shulman C, DiLavore P. Regression and word loss in autistic spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(5):936-55. 14. Meilleur AAS, Fombonne E. Regression of language and non-language

skills in pervasive developmental disorders. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2009;53(2):115-24.

15. Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Cerquiglini A, Miraglia D, Vagnoni C, Sebastianai T, et al. An investigation of sleep characteristics, EEG abnormalities and epilepsy in developmentally regressed

and non-regressed children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(10):1888-97.

16. Molloy CA, Keddache M, Martin LJ. Evidence for linkage on 21q and 7q in a subset of autism characterized by developmental regression. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(8):741-6.

17. Wiggins LD, Rice CE, Baio J. Developmental regression in children with an autism spectrum disorder identiied by a population-based surveillance system. Autism. 2009;13(4):357-74.

18. Bernabei P, Cerquiglini A, Cortesi F, D’Ardia C. Regression versus no regression in the autistic disorder: developmental trajectories. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(3):580-8.

19. Pickles A, Simonoff E, Conti-Ramsden G, Falcaro M, Simkin Z, Charman T, et al. Loss of language in early development of autism and specific language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(7):843-52.

20. Jones LA, Campbell JM. Clinical characteristics associated with language regression for children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40(1):54-62.

21. Siperstein R, Volkmar F. Parental reporting of regression in children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(6):731-4.

22. Matson JL, Wilkins J, Fodstad JC. Children with autism spectrum disorders: a comparison of those who regress vs. those who do not. Dev Neurorehabil. 2010;13(1):37-45.

23. Wilson S, Djukic A, Shinnar S, Dharmani C, Rapin I. Clinical characteristics of language regression in children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2003;45(8):508-14.

24. Werner E, Dawson G, Munson J, Osterling J. Variation in early developmental course in autism and its relation behavioral outcome at 3-4 years of age. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(3):337-50. 25. Nachmias F, Nachmias D. Research methods in the social sciences.

London: Arnold; 1996.

26. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24(5):659-85.

27. Lord C, Rutter M, Dilavore P, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 1999. 28. Hansen RL, Ozonoff S, Krakowiak P, Angkustsiri K, Jones C, Deprey

LJ, et al. Regression in autism: prevalence and associated factors in the CHARGE study. Ambul Pediatr. 2008;8(1):25-31.

29. Valente KDR, Valério RM. Transtorno invasivo do desenvolvimento e epilepsia. J Epilepsy Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;10(4 Suppl 2):41-6. 30. Backes B. Regressão da linguagem, desenvolvimento sociocomunicativo

Imagem

Table 2. Mean final scores in the investigated domains according to group
Table 3. Between-group comparison of mean socores in the investi- investi-gated dimensions  Dimensions Module 1 z (p-value)* Module 2 z (p-value)* COM -0.18 (0.66) -0.12 (0.95) INT -0.38 (0.79) -0.81 (0.45) COM/INT -0.55 (0.92) -0.55 (0.60) * Mann-Whitney

Referências

Documentos relacionados

A infestação da praga foi medida mediante a contagem de castanhas com orificio de saída do adulto, aberto pela larva no final do seu desenvolvimento, na parte distal da castanha,

dados sísmicos marítimos e em zonas de transição. Proposta de Metodologia de Avaliação Integrada de Riscos e Impactos Ambientais para Estudos de Avaliação Ambiental

Este modelo permite avaliar a empresa como um todo e não apenas o seu valor para o acionista, considerando todos os cash flows gerados pela empresa e procedendo ao seu desconto,

O conhecimento dos processos que regulam a estabilização da matéria orgânica do solo (MOS) é necessário para predizer as alterações nos estoques de C com

Despercebido: não visto, não notado, não observado, ignorado.. Não me passou despercebido

didático e resolva as ​listas de exercícios (disponíveis no ​Classroom​) referentes às obras de Carlos Drummond de Andrade, João Guimarães Rosa, Machado de Assis,

Na hepatite B, as enzimas hepáticas têm valores menores tanto para quem toma quanto para os que não tomam café comparados ao vírus C, porém os dados foram estatisticamente

É nesta mudança, abruptamente solicitada e muitas das vezes legislada, que nos vão impondo, neste contexto de sociedades sem emprego; a ordem para a flexibilização como