• Nenhum resultado encontrado

FATORES ETIOLÓGICOS

No documento Manual de Neuropsicologia (páginas 113-118)

Entre os fatores etiológicos da disfunção executiva (DE) contam-se causas muito diver- sas.

Traumatismos cranioencefálicos, especialmente por projétil de arma de fogo, os TCEs fechados (risco de lesão axonal difusa) e as contusões com hematoma intra-parenquima- toso. Crianças com traumatismo cerebral fechado grave contam-se entre estes doentes (Levin e Hanten, 2005; Wilde et al., 2005)

Epilepsias do lobos frontais, geralmente consequentes a um TCE (parciais complexas, generalizadas e Jacksonianas), fazem também parte das causas de DE.

Patologia Vascular, mais frequente em idosos por enfarte das artérias cerebral anterior, comunicante anterior, ramos sílvicos da cerebral média e porção anterior pericalosa e caloso marginal ou por rutura de aneurismas da artéria comunicante anterior.

Neoplasias, incluindo meningiomas (polares, bilaterais ou do rego olfativo), gliomas (glioblastomas bilaterais em “forma de borboleta”, oligodendrogliomas, etc.) e metásta- ses.

Esclerose Múltipla, pois as áreas frontais são região de grande implantação de placas de desmielinização (segunda área mais atingida do neuroeixo).

Doenças degenerativas, incluindo doença de Pick, doença de Alzheimer, doença de Parkinson (Dagher, Owen, Boecker e Brooks, 2001) e coreia de Huntinghton.

Patologia Infeciosa, como abcessos, herpes simplex, HIV e neurossiflis (rara).

Vários quadros psicopatológicos encontram-se também entre as causas importantes da DE. Os doentes com esquizofrenia têm demonstrado ter dificuldades em planear, contro- lar, ter flexibilidade cognitiva, na memória de trabalho, na inibição da resposta, organi- zação, fluência e monitorização.

Segundo Austin e Mitchell (1995), na depressão existe uma perturbação na rede funcio-

nal paralela que liga o pré-frontal a regiões subcorticais. Os défices executivos podem persistir depois da recuperação de uma perturbação do humor, sugerindo que pode estar implicados na sua etiologia (Austin et al., 2001).

O DE é também comum entre doentes com um síndrome de stress pós-traumático, per- turbação bipolar (Andersson, Barder, Hellvin, Løvdahl e Malt, 2008; Cahill, Walter e Ma- lhi, 2009), perturbação obsessivo-compulsiva (Watkins et al., 2005) e distúrbio de défice de atenção com hiperatividade (DDAH) (Geurts, Verte, Oosterlaan, Roeyers e Sergeant, 2004; Goldberg et al., 2005; Mahone et al., 2002), autismo (Geurts et al., 2004; Goldberg et al., 2005; Hill, 2004), síndrome de Tourette (Mahone et al., 2002; Watkins et al., 2005) e na na perturbação anti-social da personalidade (Morgan e Lilienfeld, 2000).

AVALIAÇÃO

Para a avaliação neuropsicológica do funcionamento executivo existem vários instrumen- tos, mas não existe consenso na forma de operacionalizar e medir as FE (Alvarez e Emory, 2006). Alguns testes avaliar funções específicas como a atenção, o planeamento (ou programação), a inibição ou controlo inibitório, a memória, a flexibilidade cogniti- va, a abstração, e a tomada de decisão.

Os testes que avaliam a atenção incluem os subtestes do Behavioural Inattention Test (BIT Star e Letter Cancellation time; Wilson et al., 1987); o teste das Trilhas (Reitan, 1958; Ashendorf et al., 2008; Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009); o teste de fluência verbal Fluency* (5-minute ‘s’ words; Thurstone e Thurstone, 1962); o teste Hayling (Burgess e Shallice, 1996); os subtestes da Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome battery (BADS, Wilson et al., 1996); o teste Wisconsin Card Sorting (Grant e Berg, 1948) (ver revisão de Crawford e Channon, 2002), o Teste Stroop (Castro, Cunha e Martins, 2000; Sabri, Melara e Algom, 2001), s subtestes da Bateria da Avaliação da síndrome Di- sexecutiva (Norris e Tate, 2000) o teste de Wisconsin de Classificação de Cartas (Stuss et al., 2000; Royall et al., 2002), e subteste Span de Dígitos, da Escala Wechsler de Inte- ligência para Adultos III (Wechsler, 2008).

A inibição pode medir-se através dos testes das trilhas (Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009), Teste de Hayling e Brixton (Burgess e Shallice, 1996), Labirintos de Porteus (Porteus e Diamond, 1962), o Stroop (Castro, Cunha e Martins, 2000; Sabri, Melara e Algom, 2001; Gyurak et al., 2009) e o subteste do Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire (Cummings et al., 1994).

* No sentido de avaliar a capacidade de emitir comportamentos verbais em sequência obedecendo a regas pré-estabelecidas.

O planeamento pode ser medido pelos testes das torres de Londres (Krikorian, Bartok, e Glay, 1994; Souza, Ignácio, Cunha, Oliveira, e Moll, 2001), e também o teste dos Labirin- tos (Papazian et al., 2006; Porteus e Diamond, 1962)), Teste do Relógio (Aprahamian, Martinelli, Neri, e Yassuda, 2009; Atalaia-Silva e Lourenço, 2008; Wong et al., 2004), e pelo Teste das Trilhas (Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009; Souza, Ignácio, Cunha, Oliveira, e Moll, 2001).

Para a memória, revela-se adequado o AMIPB Story Recall (Coughlan e Hollows, 1985), o PASAT que avalia a memória de trabalho, pela Figura Complexa de Rey Osterrieth (Olivei- ra, Rigoni, Andretta, e Moraes, 2004; Rey, 1988; Troyer e Wishart, 1997;); subteste Span de Dígitos da WAIS-III (Wechsler, 2008), Teste das Trilhas (Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009), Teste de Fluência Verbal (Banhato e Nascimento, 2007; Rodrigues, Yamashita, e Chiappetta, 2008).

Para a avaliação da flexibilidade cognitiva aplicam-se as séries de Luria, Teste de Hayling e Brixton (Burgess e Shallice, 1996); Teste das Trilhas (Moll, Oliveira-Souza, Moll, Bramati, e Andreiuolo, 2002; Oliveira-Souza, Ignácio, Cunha, Oliveira, e Moll, 2001;Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009) Teste de Wisconsin de Classificação de Cartas (Olivei- ra-Souza, Ignácio, Cunha, Oliveira, e Moll, 2001; Royall et al., 2002; Stuss et al., 2000). Para a avaliação do raciocínio dedutivo, podem usar-se as Matrizes progressivas de Ra- ven e as avançadas I (Raven, 1976).

Para a tomada de decisão pode aplicar-se o Teste de Wisconsin de Classificação de Car- tas (Royall et al., 2002; Stuss et al., 2000), o subteste dos cubos da WAIS-III (Wechsler, 2008), ou o Teste do Relógio (Aprahamian, Martinelli, Neri, e Yassuda, 2009).

Existem também instrumentos que avaliam várias funções, tais como a Bateria Neuropsi- cológica de Luria-Nebraska (Kashden e Franzen, 1996; Maia et al., 2005; McKinzey, Podd, Krehbiel, Mensch, e Trombka, 1997) e a Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB, Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan, e Pillon, 2000).

Bibliografia

Alvarez, J. A. e Emory, E. (2006). Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: A Meta-Analytic Re- view. Neuropsychology Review,16(1), 17-42. DOI: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x

Andersson, S., Barder, H. E., Hellvin, T., Løvdahl, H. e Malt, U. F. (2008). Neuropsychological and electrophysiological indices of neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar II disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 10, 888-899.

Andrés, P. (2003). Frontal cortex as the central executive of working memory: time to revise our view. Cortex, 39(4–5), 871–895.

Aprahamian, I., Martinelli, J. E., Neri, A. L., e Yassuda, M. S. (2009). The Clock Drawing Test: A review of its accuracy in screening for dementia. Dementia e Neuropsychologia, 3(2), 74-80. Aron, A. R, Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J. e Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stopsignal inhi- bition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 115-116.

Ashendorf, L., Jefferson, A. L., O’Connor, M. K., Chaisson, C., Robert, C. G., e Robert, A. S. (2008). Trail Making Test errors in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. Archives of Clini- cal Neuropsychology, 23, 129-137. doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.11.005.

Atalaia-Silva, K. C., e Lourenço, R. A. (2008). Tradução, adaptação e validação de construto do Teste do Relógio aplicado entre idosos no Brasil. Revista Saúde Pública, 42(5), 930-937.

Baddeley, A. D. (2002). The psychology of memory. Em A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman e B. A. Wilson (Eds.), The essential handbook of memory disorders for clinicians (2ª Ed., pp. 3- 15). Chi- chester, West Sussex: Wiley.

Badgaiyan, R. D. (2002). Executive control, willed actions, and nonconscious processing. Human Brain Mapping, 9, 38–41. Bočková, M., Chládek, J., Jurák, P., Halámek, J. e Rektor, I. (2007). Exe- cutive functions processed in the frontal and lateral temporal cortices: Intracerebral study. Clini- cal Neurophysiology, 118(12), 2625-2636.

Barcelo, F. e Knight, R. T. (2002). Both random and perseverative errors underlie WCST deficits in prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia, 40, 349–356.

Barcelo, F. e Rubia, F. J. (1998). Non-frontal P3b-like activity evoked by the Wisconsin Card Sor- ting Test. Neuroreport, 9, 747–751.

Barkley, R. A. (1996). Linkages between attention and executive functions. Em G. R. Lyon e N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 307–325). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: cons- tructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psycholy Bulletin, 121, 65-94.

Bechara, A, Damasio, H. e Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295-307.

Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects of frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6, 53-60.

Bjork, R. A., Bjork, E. L. e Anderson, M. C. (1998). Varieties of goal-directed forgetting. Em J. M. Golding, C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 103-137). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Ernslie, H., Evans, J. e Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 547– 558.

Burgess, P. W., e Shallice, T. (1996). Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following

frontal lobes lesion. Neuropsychologia, 34, 263-276.

Cabeza, R. e Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and FMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences,12, 1-47.

Cahill, C. M., Walter, G. e Malhi, G. S. (2009). Neurocognition in bipolar disorder and juvenile bipolar disorder. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 18(3), 221- 230.

Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N. e Fossella, J. (2002). Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approa- ches toward a model of cognitive control. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 237-254.

Castro, S. L., Cunha, L. S., e Martins, L. (2000). Teste Stroop Neuropsicológico em Português. A c e d i d o e m 2 7 , F e v e r e i r o , 2 0 1 0 , e m

http://repositorio.up.pt/aberto/bitstream/10216/9191/2/Teste%20Stroop%20Neuropsicolgico%20 em%20Portugus.pdf.

Channon, S. e Crawford, S. (1999). Problem-solving in real-life-type situations: The effects of anterior and posterior lesions on performance. Neuropsychologia, 37, 757-770.

Crawford, S. e Channon, S. (2002). Dissociation between performance on abstract tests of executi- ve function and problem solving in real-life-type situations in normal aging. Aging e Mental Health, 6(1), 12-21.

Cummings J. e Mega, M. (2003). Neuropsychiatry and behavioral neuroscience. Nova Iorque: Oxford Press

Cummings, J. L., Mega, M., Gray, K., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., Carusi, D. A., e Gombein, J. (1994). The Neuropsychiatry Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in demen- tia. Neurology, 44, 2308-2314.

Dagher, A., Owen, A. M., Boecker, H. e Brooks, D. J. (2001). The role of the striatum and hippo- campus in planning: A PET activation study in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 124, 1020–1032.

Darby, D. e Walsh, K. (2005). Walsh's neuropsychology: A clinical approach (5ª ed.). Nova Iorque: Elsevier.

Denckla, M. B. (1994).Measurement of executive function. Em G. R. Lyon e N. A. Kostovic, I., Ska- vic, J. e Strinovic, D. (1988) Acetyl cholinesterase in the human frontal associative cortex during the period of cognitive development: early laminar shifts and late innervation of pyramidal neu- rons. Neurosciences Letters, 90(1-2), 107-112.

Diamond A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cog- nitive function, anatomy, and biochemistry. Em D. T. Stuss e R. T. Kinight (Eds.), Principles of fron- tal lobe function (pp. 466-503). London: Oxford University Press

Dunbar, K. e Sussman, D. (1995). Toward a cognitive account of frontal lobe function: Simulating frontal lobe deficits in normal subjects. Em J. Grafman, K. J. Holyoak e F. Boller (Eds.), Structu- re and functions of the human prefrontal cortex. Annals of the New YorkAcademy of Sciences (Vol. 769, pp. 289–304). New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.

Eriksen, B. A. e Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Percept Psychophys, 16, 143-149.

Gerardi-Caulton, G. (2000). Sensitivity to spatial conflict and the development of self-regulation in children 24-36 months of age. Developmental Science, 3, 397-404.

Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. e Mangun, G. R. (2002). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind (2.ª Ed.). Scranton, Pennsylvania: W.W. Norton

Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J. e Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 3 1/2-7 years old on a Strooplike day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129-153. Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H. e Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 836-854.

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C. e Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executi- ve function. Child Neuropsychology (Neuropsychology, Development and Cognition: Section C), 6, 235-238.

Godefroy, O. (2003). Frontal syndrome and disorders of executive functions. Journal of Neuro- logy, 250, 1–6.

Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M., Astor, B. C., Denckla, M. B. e Landa, R. J. (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism Develeopmental Disorders, 35, 279-293.

Golstein, K. e Sheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete behavior: an experimental study with special tests. Psychological Monographs, 53(2), 1-151.

Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490-501.

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996). Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 13473-13480.

Hartman-Maeir, A., e Katz, N. (1995). Validity of the Behavioral Inattention Test (BIT): rela- tionships with functional tasks. School of Occupational Therapy, Hebrew University, 49(6), 507- 516.

Kashden, J. e Franzen, M. D. (1996). An interrater reliability study of the Luria-Nebraska Neu- ropsychological Battery Form-II quantitative scoring system. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11(2), 155-163.

Kimble, D. P. (1963). Physiological psychology: a unit for introductory psychology (pp. 286-287). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley

Krikorian, R., Bartok, J., e Glay, N. (1994). Tower of London procedure: A standard method and developmental data. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 840-850.

Kringelback, M. L., de Araujo, I. E. T. e Rolls, E. T. (2004). Taste-related activity in the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 21(2), 781-788.

Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 26-32.

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B. e Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4ª Ed., pp.35-36). Nova Iorque: Oxford University Press.

Levin, H. S. e Hanten, G. (2005). Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatric Neurology, 33, 79-93.

Logan, G. D, Cowan, W. B. e Davis, K. A. (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reacti- on time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Percepti- on and Performance, 10 276-91.

Mahone, E. M., Cirino, P. T., Cutting, L. E., Cerrone, P. M., Hagelthorn, K. M., Hiemenz, J. R., ... Denckla, M. B. (2002). Validity of the behavior rating inventory of executive function in children with ADHD and/or Tourette syndrome. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 643-662.

Miller, E. K. e Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24,167-202.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A. e Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

Morgan, A. B. e Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20,113–136.

Norris, G., e Tate, R. L. (2000). The behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome (BADS):

ecological, concurrent and construct validity. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10(1), 33-45. Oliveira, M., Rigoni, M., Andretta, I., e Moraes, J. F. (2004). Validação do Teste Figuras Complexas de Rey na população brasileira. Avaliação Psicológica, 3(1), 33-38.

Papazian, O., Alfonso, I. e Luzondo, R. J. (2006). Trastornos de as funciones ejecutivas. Revista de Neurología, 42 (S3), S-45-S50.

Passolunghi, M. C. e Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory con- trol in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 44-57.

Pennington, B. F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, O. e Roberts, R. J. (1996). Executive functions and working memory: Theoretical and measurement issues. Em G. R. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 117–142). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. Lyon e N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 327–348). Baltimo- re, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.

Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F., Bramati, I., e Andreiuolo, P.(2002). The cerebral correlates of set- shifting: An fMRI study of the trail making test. Arquivo Neuropsiquiatria, 60(4), 900-905. Porteus, S. D., e Diamond, A. I. (1962). Porteus Maze Changes after psychosurgery. Journal of Mental Science, 108, 53-58. doi: 10.1192/bjp.108.452.53 1962.

Rabbitt, P. (1997). Introduction: Methodologies and models in the study of executive function. Em P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 1–38). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Reeve, R. A. e Brown, A. L. (1985). Metacognition reconsidered: implications for intervention re- search. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 343-356.

Rey, A. (1988). Teste de cópia de figuras complexas André Rey (1º Ed.). Lisboa: CEGOC-TEA. Robbins, T. W. (2000). Chemical neuromodulation of frontal-executive functions in humans and other animals. Experimental Brain Research, 133, 130-138.

Rodrigues, A. B., Yamashita, E. T., Chiappetta, A. l. (2008).Teste de fluência verbal no adulto e no idoso: verificação da aprendizagem verbal. Revista CEFAC, 10(4), 443-451.

Rolls, E. T. (2004). Development of orbitofrontal function. Brain e Cognition, 55(1), 11-29.

Royall, D. R., Lauterbach, E. C., Cummings, J. L., Reeve, A., Rummans, T. A., Kaufer, D. I., ... Coffey, C. E.(2002). Executive control function: A review of its promise and challenges for clinical research. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14(4), 377-405.

Royall, D. R., Mahurin, R. K., e Gray, K. F., (1992). Bedside Assessment of Executive Cognitive Impairment. The EXIT Interview. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40, 1221-1226. Sabri, M., Melara, R.D., e Algom, D. (2001). A confluence of contexts: asymetric versus global failures of selective attention to stoop dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 515-537. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.515.

Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E. B., Aase, H. e Russell, V. A. (2005). A dynamic developmental theory of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and com- bined subtypes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 397-419.

Sánchez-Cubillo, I., Periañez, J. A., Adrover-Roig, D., Rodríguez-Sánchez, J. M., Ríos-Lago, M.,Tirapu, J., e Barceló, F. (2009). Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: Role of task-swi- tching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 438-450. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090626.

Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H. e Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functio- ning for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130, 3–28.

Souza, R. O., Ignácio, F. A., Cunha, F. C., Oliveira, D. L., e Moll, J. (2001). Contribuição à neurop- sicologia do comportamento executivo: Torre de Londres e Teste de Wisconsin em indivíduos nor-

mais Arquivo Neuropsiquiatria, 59(3-A), 526-531.

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jernigan, T. L. e Toga, A. W. (1999). In vivo eviden- ce for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions. Nature neurosciences, 2(10), 859-861.

Starkstein, S. E., Mayberg, H. S., Berthier, M. L., Fedoroff, P., Price, T. R., Dannals, R. F., ... Ro- binson, R. G. (1990). Mania after brain injury: neuroradiological and metabolic findings. Annals of Neurology, 27, 652-659.

Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652-654.

Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Hamer, L., Palumbo, C., Dempster, R., Binns, M., Levine, B. e Izuka- wa, D. (1998). The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 265–278.

Stuss, D. T., Floden, D., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B. e Katz, D. (2001). Stroop performance in fo- cal lesion patients: Dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 39, 771–786.

Stuss, D.T., Levine, B., Alexander, M.P., Hong, J., Palumbo, C., Harmer, L., ... e Izukawad, D. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and poste¬rior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neu- ropsychologia, 38(4), 388-402.doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00093-7.

Tranel, D. e Damasio, A. R. (2002). Neurobiological foundations. Em A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopel- man e B. A. Wilson (Eds.), The essential handbook of memory disorders for clinicians (2ª Ed., pp. 17-56). Chichester, West Sussex: Wiley.

Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., and Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsycho- logia, 36, 499–504.

Troyer, A. K., e Wishart, H. A. (1997). A comparison of qualitative scoring systems for the Rey-Os- terrieth Complex Figure Test. The clinical Neuropsychologist, 11(49), 381-390.

Watkins, L. H., Sahakian, B. J., Robertson, M. M., Veale, D. M., Rogers, R. D., Pickard, K. M., ... Robbins, T. W. (2005). Executive function in Tourette’s syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disor- der. Psychological Medicine, 35, 571-582.

Wechsler, D. (2008). WAIS III: Manual da escala de Inteligência de Wechsler para Adultos – Tercei- ra edição. Lisboa: CEGOC-TEA.

Welsh, M. C. (2002). Developmental and clinical variations in executive functions. Em D. L. Molfe- se e V. J. Molfese (Eds.), Developmental variations in learning: Applications to social, executive function, language, and reading skills (pp. 139–185). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Welsch, M. C. e Pennington, B. F. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 199–230.

Welsch, M. C., Pennington, B. F. e Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative-developmental study of exe- cutive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131–149.

Wilde, E. A., Hunter, J. V., Newsome, M. R., Scheibel, R. S., Bigler, E. D. , Johnson, J. L., ... Le- vin, H. S. (2005). Frontal and temporal morphometric findings on MRI in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 22, 333-344.

Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D. e Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 35, 205-213.

Wong, A., Mok, V. C., Yim, P., Fu, M., Lam, W. W., Yau, C., ... e Wong, K. S. (2004). The executive clock drawing task (CLOX) is a poor screening test for executive dysfunction in Chinese elderly patients with subcortical ischemic vascular disease. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 11(5), 493-

497. doi:10.1016/j.jocn.2004.03.005.

Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S. F. e Feeney, T. J. (1998). Cognitive rehabilitation: executive functions. Em M. Ylvisaker (Ed.), Traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: children and adolescents (2.ª Ed., pp. 221-270). Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann

Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S. e Frye, D. (1997). Early development of executive functi- on: A problem-solving framework. Review of General Psychology, 1, 198–226.

•Esquizofrenia

No documento Manual de Neuropsicologia (páginas 113-118)